Conference on Comparative Cognition 2005 - Spoken
Presenations |
|
|
Acerbo |
Martin J. Acerbo (University of
Michigan) and Juan D. Delius (Universität Konstanz) |
Sensitization
to Apomorphine in the Pigeons: Effects of Dizocilpine NMDA Receptor Blockade |
The
dopamine agonist apomorphine (Apo) elicits context-specific sensitization in
pigeons. We investigated whether this learning involves
dopamino-glutamatergic interactions by inducing NMDA receptor blockade with
the antagonist dizocilpine (Diz). In the first experiment, we found that 0.10
mg/kg of Diz co-administered with a standard dose of 0.5 mg/kg of Apo
impaired both the development and the expression of the conditioned pecking
response. A second experiment found that only Diz plus Apo co-treatment
affected the sensitization; the presentation of Diz alone had no effect. The
last experiment examined whether the administration of Diz had an immediate
effect on the Apo sensitization and on the conditioned response shown by
already sensitized pigeons. First treatment produced no effect, but there was
a marked response inhibition with second treatment. Thus, NMDA receptors play an important role
in Apo-induced sensitization in pigeons that is compatible with the Pavlovian
conditioning account of sensitization. |
Adachi |
Ikuma Adachi (Kyoto
University Japan Society for the
Promotion of Science) |
Cross-modal representations in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri
sciureus) |
In
present study, we tested two squirrel monkeys with a symbolic matching to
sample procedure. In the training phase, we trained them to discriminate
photographs of two caretakers of them. After reaching the criterion, they
were exposed to two test sessions. In these sessions, 32 all-reinforced test
trials were interspersed among the training trials. In the test trials, a
voice, either matching(match condition) or mismatching(mismatch condition) to
the sample photos, was played back after the sample stimulus disappeared.
Their performances in mismatch condition were significantly lower than that
in match condition. This suggests that our subjects have recalled the
caretaker’s representation upon hearing his/her voice, and that broke in on
the subjects’ memory of the preceded sample stimulus in mismatch condition. |
Bateson |
Melissa Bateson (University of
Newcastle upon Tyne) |
Can
energetic state explain apparent rationality violations in hummingbird
foraging decisions? |
Hummingbirds’
relative preferences for two flower types differing in nectar volume and
concentration are altered by the addition of a third, less profitable flower
type. We have interpreted this apparently irrational behaviour as evidence
that hummingbirds use comparative evaluation mechanisms when making foraging
decisions. However, Schuck-Paim, Pompilio & Kacelnik (2004) have argued
that the change in preference we observed may be reinterpreted as a rational
response to a change in energetic state caused by the introduction of the
third flower type. We test this hypothesis by analysing the rate of energy
intake of hummingbirds faced with two versus three flower types. We show that
hummingbirds compensate for foraging bouts resulting in greater energetic
intake with longer subsequent inter-bout intervals. This behaviour has the
effect of equalising long-term rate of energy intake in the two and three
flower treatments. Therefore, our data do not support the state-dependent
hypothesis in the hummingbird system. |
Bauer |
Gordon B. Bauer (New College of
Florida), Joseph C. Gaspard III, Debborah E. Colbert, Jennifer B. Leach (Mote
Marine Laboratory), Roger Reep (University of Florida) |
Tactile
Discrimination of Textures by Florida Manatees, Trichechus manatus
latirostris |
Knowledge
of the sensory abilities of the endangered Florida manatee is important for
management and conservation of the species.
As part of a program to broadly survey the manatee’s sensory
abilities, we studied discrimination of textures by two males in a two-choice
discrimination task. A modified
staircase method was used. Stimuli consisted of acrylic plates with vertical
gratings of ridges and grooves. The
standard stimulus, present on every trial, had 2 mm gratings and the
comparison stimuli had wider gratings.
The blindfolded subjects were trained to demonstrate discrimination by
pressing the target with wider gratings. Performance at a level of
seventy-five percent correct indicated the discrimination threshold. Ongoing testing currently suggests a
threshold of 2.18 mm for both subjects.
This threshold corresponds to a Weber fraction of .09, which is
somewhat lower than the closely related Antillean manatee, Trichechus
manatus manatus, and similar to the harbor
seal. |
Blaisdell |
Aaron P. Blaisdell (University
of California, Los Angeles), Kosuke Sawa (Japan Society for the Promotion of
Science, Nagoya University; University of California, Los Angeles), &
Michael Waldmann (University of Göttingen, Germany) |
Seeing versus
doing: Two modes of assessing causal models by rats |
Waldmann
and Hagmayer (in press) found that people make different predictions from an
observationally-acquired causal model, depending on whether they believe that
an event within the model has been merely observed (“seeing”) or was actively
manipulated (“doing”). In a replication with rats, subjects received three
types of training trials: a) Stimulus A followed by Stimulus X (i.e., A
--> X); b) Stimulus A followed by sucrose (i.e., A --> sucrose); and c)
simultaneous presentations of Stimulus Y and sucrose (i.e., Y+sucrose).
Subsequently, levers were made available and Stimulus X or Y was delivered
either contingent on lever pressing (Doing) or noncontingently (Seeing).
Reduced nosepoking was observed to Stimulus X in the Doing versus the Seeing
condition, while the rate of nosepoking to Stimulus Y was similar in both
conditions. This replicates the effect demonstrated in humans and suggests
that rats discount a previously established cause (A) when presented with an
intervening cause (lever pressing). |
Bloomfield |
Laurie L. Bloomfield, Tiffany
T-Y. Lee, Marc T. Avey & Christopher B. Sturdy (University of Alberta) |
Call-based
species classification by black-capped and mountain chickadees |
Black-capped
and mountain chickadees were trained in an operant discrimination task using
exemplars of each species chick-a-dee calls as discriminative stimuli to
determine: (1) whether birds memorized or classified the calls of
conspecifics and heterospecifics and (2) for black-capped chickadees, whether
experience with heterospecific chick-a-dee calls improved their performance.
The task consisted of two simultaneous discriminations. For the
within-category discrimination, calls of one species served as S+ (rewarded)
and S- (unrewarded) stimuli. For the between-category discrimination, calls
of the other species served as S-s. Overall, birds discriminated between call
categories faster than within a call category, and in two subsequent
experiments, birds showed transfer to novel calls and, following a
contingency reversal procedure, propagation back to between-category calls.
The results provide converging evidence that the acoustically similar calls
of these two chickadee species constitute separate open-ended categories and
are perceived as such by members of each species, regardless of experience. |
Bodily |
Bodily, K. D. & Sturz, B. R.
(Auburn University) |
Virtual
open-field: Evidence for integration of spatial maps in humans and pigeons? |
An
interactive 3-D computer-generated analogue of the Blaisdell & Cook
(2005) open-field task was constructed for humans. Participants used keyboard keys and a mouse
to search a virtual environment for a goal hidden in 1 of 16 raised
cups. In Phase 1, the goal was
consistently located between two landmarks, a blue T and a red L. In Phase 2,
the goal was consistently located down and left of a blue T. To test for
integration of spatial information, the red L was presented alone with no
goal following two Phase 2 trials (Test 1) or a Phase 1 and a Phase 2 trial
(Test 2). Results did not differ from those found with pigeons, suggesting
integration of spatial information.
Additional analyses, however, suggested that an accumulation of
non-reinforced choice responses resulted in a shift in search behavior across
tests that was not accounted for by integration. |
Bohn |
Sandra Bohn & Stan Kuczaj
(University of Southern Mississippi) |
Vocal Development in a Bottlenose Dolphin Calf: Examples
of Imitation and Possible Vocal Play |
Imitation
and vocal play are an important part of vocal development in many
species. The vocalizations of a
stranded bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) calf were recorded over the
course of six months after she was introduced into a captive population of
bottlenose dolphins. The calf imitated
both the trainer’s whistle and the signature whistle of another dolphin. The calf also appeared to be engaging in
vocal play similar to babbling found in human infants. On multiple occasions she produced a series
of whistles with different contours which could be analogous to an infant
producing a series of different syllables.
All examples were recorded when the calf was swimming alone. These examples suggest that vocal play may
play a role in the vocal development of bottlenose dolphins. |
Braaten |
Richard F. Braaten (Colgate
University) |
Song
Memory During the Sensitive Period in Zebra Finches |
Previous
studies of bird song learning have relied primarily on song production or
operant discrimination procedures to assess song memory. However, song
production assays confound memory with other cognitive processes that may be
involved in song learning, and operant discrimination procedures do not
necessarily reveal what birds would learn in the absence of reward and
punishment. In this study we exposed
male and female juvenile zebra finches to conspecific and heterospecific
songs presented through a loudspeaker over a period of nine days during their
sensitive period for song learning.
After song exposure, birds were trained to discriminate a subset of
the songs in a novel vs. familiar operant discrimination task. Probe testing
with untrained songs revealed that both males and females memorize songs
presented passively through a loudspeaker, they memorize both conspecific and
heterospecific songs, and they memorize songs that are not later used for
production. |
Brooks |
Daniel Brooks (Tufts
University), Kazuhiro Goto (University of Nebraska-Lincoln), Robert Cook
(Tufts University) |
Emergent
Perceptual Structure in Pigeons |
In
order to attempt to isolate and understand the mechanisms responsible for
processing emergent perceptual structure, pigeons were tested in a
two-alternative choice task that required global integration of organized
local information. Stimuli consisted of two random distractor backgrounds
generated from black and white square elements, one of which contained a
structured target (e.g., stripes, checkerboard, squares). These stimuli were
tested at four levels of spatial granularity (i.e., element sizes).
Experiments using new targets made of randomly generated “mosaic” patterns
examined the roles of repetition and symmetry in globally mediating target
localization. Results indicate that pigeons can perceive and discriminate
emergent visual structure and suggest they do so using a generalized
pattern-detection rule rather than a rule for recognizing stimulus specific
elements present in the original targets. |
Brown |
Stephanie Brown, Stan Kuczaj
(University of Southern Mississippi), & Moby Solangi (Institute for
Marine Mammal Studies) |
Social Learning Facilitates the Development of Play Behavior in a
Bottlenose Dolphin Calf |
A
wild-born bottlenose dolphin calf was stranded and rescued at approximately
six months of age. When this calf was
first introduced to a group of captive dolphins, both she and the captive
animals evidenced considerable interest in one another. The dolphin calf often oriented at other
dolphins, and slowly learned to engage in behaviors similar to those she
observed. For example, the calf
initially avoided contact with balls left in her pool, but did observe other
dolphins play with balls. As she
became integrated into this group of dolphins, the calf began to interact
with balls in ways similar to those she had observed. These observations suggest that social
learning influenced the ball play of this dolphin. |
Brown |
Michael F. Brown, Robert Farley,
Haley Solodky, & Rachel Bachrach (Villanova University) |
Oh, The
Places You've Been!: Social Memories for Spatial Locations |
We will
present preliminary evidence suggesting that choices in laboratory spatial
tasks (the Radial-arm Maze and the Pole Box) can be controlled by the
locations of choices make earlier in the trial by another rat. This control is apparently based on
observations of the choices made by the other rat. It can be expressed as either an increase
or a decrease in the probability of visiting locations previouly visited by
the other rat. This control is
interpreted in terms of working memory for the choice behavior of a foraging
companion. Such memories increase
foraging efficiency and might be expected in animals that forage socially. |
Caetano |
Marcelo Caetano (Brown
University) |
Factors
Affecting the Speed of Learning of Temporal Discriminations |
The
speed of learning temporal discriminations depends on the training procedure
used. Training different intervals in a multiple cued interval (MCI)
procedure produces faster learning when one of the intervals is changed
daily, compared to when the interval is held constant. The goal of this study
was to determine whether daily changes contribute to faster learning, and to
identify procedural factors involved, such as amount of training and
transfer. Twelve rats were trained with two constant intervals (30 and 120
sec), and one interval that varied daily (30, 60 or 120 sec). The speed of
learning of the temporal intervals was assessed. In a second experiment, four
groups were trained with different proportions of the changed interval (1,
.67, .33 and 0), and the speed of learning under each condition was assessed.
The results suggest that the faster learning occurred because of positive
transfer from the amount of overall training. |
Castro |
Leyre Castro, Olga F. Lazareva,
Shaun P. Vecera, & Edward A. Wasserman (University of Iowa) |
Figure-Ground
assignment in pigeons: The effect of
different sizes |
One of
the well-known cues for figure-ground assignment in humans is area: smaller regions are more likely to be
perceived as figure than larger regions.
Are also pigeons sensitive to this cue? After training our pigeons to discriminate
whether a small target spot appeared on a colored figural shape or on a
differently colored background, we varied the figure’s size so that it could
be smaller or larger that the trained figure.
As the size of the figure was reduced, pigeon’s performance
improved: accuracy was higher on both
figure and background trials. In
addition, although pigeons were faster to detect the target when it appeared
on the figure in all of the conditions, when the figure was small they were
even faster in detecting the target on the figure and even slower in
detecting the target on the background. |
Cheng |
Ken Cheng (Macquarie University) |
Pigeon
spatial cognition: some common principles |
Spatial
cognition has been a long standing interest of Bill Roberts. I started work
on spatial cognition in pigeons as a postdoctoral fellow with Bill. I present
here four themes that stemmed from that work, themes common to many areas of
animal cognition. |
Theme 1:
quantity representation - Pigeons seem to represent vectors to landmarks, a
form of quantitative representation. |
Theme 2:
generalization and discrimination - In the spatial domain, pigeons exhibit
both spatial generalization and peak shift.
Generalization obeys Roger Shepard’s universal law. |
Theme 3:
averaging multiple sources - Often, information from different sources is
averaged in a weighted fashion to direct behavior. Sometimes, even information from different
domains of experience (e.g., spatial and temporal dictates) can be averaged. |
Theme 4:
cue competition - Cue competition, such as blocking and overshadowing, is
sometimes found. |
Church |
Russell M. Church & Paulo
Guilhardi (Brown University) |
A Turing
Test of a Timing Theory |
A
quantitative theory of timing or conditioning can be evaluated with a Turing
test in which the behavioral results of an experiment can be compared with
the predicted results from the theory. An example is described based upon an
experiment in which 12 rats were trained on three fixed-interval schedules of
reinforcement, and a simulation of the predicted results from a Packet Theory
of Timing. An objective classification rule was used to determine whether a
sample from the data or a sample from the theory was more similar to another
sample from the theory. The probability of a correct classification was 0.6.
This was substantially better than the worst possible theory in which correct
classification would always occur (1.0), but reliably worse than an ideal
theory in which correct classification would occur at chance (0.5). A Turing
test provides a graded metric for the evaluation of a quantitative theory. |
Clayton |
Nicola Clayton, Nathen Emery
& Anthony Dickinson (University of Cambridge) |
Mental
Time Travel by Food-Caching Western Scrub-Jays (Aphelocoma californica):
Prospective Cognition |
The
mental time travel hypothesis claims that only humans are capable of
retrospective cognition by travelling backwards in time to recollect specific
past events (episodic memory) and prospective cognition by travelling
forwards in time to anticipate future needs (future planning). Our studies of
food caching by Western Scrub-Jays have challenged the retrospective
component by demonstrating that cache recovery is mediated by an
episodic-like memory for the caching episode. We have also investigated the
prospective component by demonstrating that the jays can anticipate the
condition of their caches at future recoveries on the basis of past
recoveries and adjust their caching strategies accordingly. Moreover, the
jays engage in a number of cache protection strategies if – and only if -
another bird has observed the caching event. Taken together, these results
suggest that jays can take action now for future eventualities on the basis
of past experience (future planning). |
Cohen |
Jerome Cohen, Sean Nicholson,
Tim Frigon (University of Windsor) |
Blind Rats Bluff or Testing Parallel Map Theory in the
Enclosed Radial Arm Maze |
Parallel
Map Theory (Jacobs & Schenk, 2003) maintains that organisms only
effectively use spatial maps of local landmarks when they integrate them with
a directional map from distal landmarks or a compass cue. We test this idea
in the enclosed 8-arm radial maze in which rats were made ’blind’ to the
direction of proximally-cued arms in the maze after being able to see all
arms during initial training. During
subsequent “blind” training, rats had to open an opaque door to enter arms
with all doors closed at the beginning of each run. As expected, rats exposed
to a consistent configuration of arms were no more accurate as measured by
re-entries (macro-choices) but made fewer partial entries of only opening a
door (micro-choices) than rats exposed to varying configurations over trials.
Rotating arms or varying their configuration within a trial during
post-training probe trials also only disrupted the consistent group’s
performance as predicted. To further test Parallel Map Theory (PMT), we are
adding a specific directional cue in decision chamber which should only
enhance the consistent group’s performance. We also present data from this
manipulation. |
Cook |
Robert Cook, Richard Chechile,
& Daniel Brooks (Tufts University) |
Wait!
Wait! Don’t Tell Me |
The
working memory of pigeons was tested using concurrent recognition and a
recall-analog tasks. Multinomial process tree modeling was used to obtain
estimates of successful retrieval, sufficient storage, partial storage, and
no storage of sample information. Further, on probe trials, pigeons were
given additional opportunities to make correct choices after initially
committing an error. These post-error second and third choices were well
above chance. The choice and memory processes critical to these different
effects are presented and the implications for understanding the structure of
avian memory systems discussed. |
Crystal |
Jonathon D. Crystal (University
of Georgia) |
Short-Interval
Timing is Based on a Self-Sustaining Endogenous Oscillator |
A
defining feature of a circadian oscillator is that periodic output from the
oscillator continues after the cessation of periodic input. In contrast, a defining feature of a
pacemaker-accumulator system is that elapsed time is measured with respect to
the presentation of a stimulus; consequently, the output of a short-interval
system is periodic if presented with periodic input. However, periodic output ceases if the
periodic input is discontinued. Rats
were trained to time short intervals (e.g., 96 seconds); periodic delivery of
food produced periodic behavior. Next,
delivery of food was suspended.
Behavior was periodic after the termination of periodic input. These data suggest that short-interval
timing is based on a self-sustaining, endogenous oscillator. |
de Kort |
Selvino R. de Kort, Lucie
Salwiczek, Anthony Dickinson and Nicola S. Clayton (University of Cambridge) |
The
prospective cognition of caching |
The
recovery of caches by Western scrub-jays shows aspects of retrospective
mental time travel (MTT). According to the MTT hypothesis retrospective and
prospective cognition are controlled by common processes and so predicts that
caching should also be under cognitive control. In two experiments we tested
whether the scrub jays are sensitive to the future state of their caches. In
the first experiment the jays rapidly learned to avoid caching in the
location where their caches degraded, suggesting that they are sensitive to
the future states of their caches.
However, in a second experiment, we found that as long as the jays
were able to recover fresh food items, they continued caching this type of
food even though their own caches were consistently degraded at recovery.
This finding suggests that caching may not be directly controlled by the
expectation about the state of specific caches at recovery. |
DeLong |
Caroline M. DeLong (Brown
University), Whitlow W. Au (Hawaii Institute of Marine Biology), & Sarah
Stamper (New College of Florida) |
Echo
Features Used by Human Listeners and Echolocating Dolphins to Discriminate
Among Cylinders with Different Wall Thicknesses |
Echolocating
dolphins extract object feature information from the acoustic parameters of
echoes. However, little is known about which object features are salient to
dolphins or how they extract those features.
To gain insight into how dolphins might be extracting feature
information, human listeners can be presented (via headphones) with echoes
from the objects used in a dolphin’s discrimination task. In two recent studies, human listeners were
able to identify objects varying in size, shape, and material using echoes (M
= 88% correct) and reported using cues such as loudness, pitch, and
timbre. In this study, human listeners
were presented with echoes from hollow cylinders with varying wall
thicknesses (+/- 0.8 mm). The dolphin
was able to discriminate between cylinders that varied by 0.3 mm. The cues
reported by the human participants in this task will give insight into the
types of cues that were available to the dolphin. |
Dickinson |
Ulrike Klossek & Anthony
Dickinson (University of Cambridge) |
Assessing
Goal-Directed Action in Children by Outcome Devaluation |
Contemporary
learning theory assumes that instrumental behavior is controlled conjointly
by two processes, one mediating habitual responding and the other mediating
goal-directed action. We investigated development of instrumental behavior by
training children to perform two responses for different outcomes. The
responses were the manipulation of stimuli on a touch screen, whereas the
outcomes were the presentation of brief, animated cartoon clips. Following
instrumental training, one of the cartoons was devalued by repetitive
presentations to induce specific satiety in the absence of the opportunity to
perform the instrumental responses. When subsequently tested in extinction,
the older (> 27 months), but not the younger (< 27 moths) children performed
the response trained with the valued outcome more than that trained with
devalued outcome. In the case of arbitrary response-outcome contingencies,
the capacity for goal-directed action appears to develop between 2-3 yr of
age with instrumental responding by younger children being primarily
habitual. |
DiGian |
Kelly A. DiGian & Thomas
Zentall (University of Kentucky) |
When
pigeons are uncertain, how do they choose? |
In two
experiments, pigeons were trained with matching-to-sample to match a frequent
sample (80% of the trials), S1, to one comparison, C1, and an infrequent
sample (20% of the trials), S2, to another comparison, C2. Number of
reinforcements was equated by reinforcing 25% of responses to C1 and 100% of
responses to C2. Thus, ideally there would be 20 reinforcements associated
with each comparison but C1 would be chosen four times as often as C2. A
delay was inserted to determine whether under conditions of uncertainty
pigeons would choose based on the number of reinforcements per comparison
(they should be indifferent), number of comparison choices during training
(preference for C1), or the probability of reinforcement given a response
(preference for C2). The results suggest that pigeons chose the comparison
associated with the frequent sample (C1). Thus when uncertain, the pigeons
chose the comparison that had been chosen most often. |
Dorrance |
Brigette Dorrance, Andrew Meyer,
Karen Mason, & Jennifer Polcyn (Augustana College) |
Two-Action Observation by Budgerigars: Imitation or
Affordance Learning? |
Experiments
testing for imitation in animals often fail to control for the possibility
that animals can learn to perform a behavior by watching the movement of the
object being manipulated (affordance learning), rather than learning to
imitate the specific behavior of the demonstrator. In this experiment, budgerigars observed
demonstrator budgerigars use their beaks to remove a stopper from a food
container, either by pulling up or pushing down. To test for affordance learning, another
group observed an experimenter using fishing line to move the stopper up or
down. Subjects that observed the
demonstration by a budgerigar showed a significant tendency to remove the
stopper using the same method, whereas subjects that observed the stopper
move with the fishing line did not. As
the budgerigars failed to show any evidence of affordance learning, imitative
learning is the most likely explanation. |
Fiset |
Sylvain Fiset & Valérie
Leblanc (Université de Moncton, campus d'Edmundston) |
Object permanence in domestic dogs: The influence of the experimenter |
The
purpose of this study was to determine how the presence of an experimenter
influences the search behaviour of dogs in visible and invisible displacement
problems of object permanence. On visible condition, the dog saw the
experimenter behind the hiding boxes. On hidden condition, a large panel hid
the experimenter. On both conditions, two types of problems were given:
visible and invisible displacement of object. On visible displacement
problems, the performance was very high. On invisible displacement problems,
however, the performance was slightly over chance and it was higher in the
visible than in the hidden condition. The analysis of errors also revealed
that the dogs searched primarily as a function of the transport container in
the invisible displacement problems. This experiment suggests that dogs do
not understand invisible displacement problems and that the presence of the
experimenter increases the performance of dogs in invisible displacement
problems. |
Gibson |
Brett Gibson, Michelle
Leichtman, Mike Simpson & Debbie Kung (University of New Hampshire) |
Use of geometric and featural cues during a 2-D search
task by adults and children |
Adults
and children were presented with an array of three landmarks on a computer
screen. A cartoon type character “hid” behind one of the landmarks during the
first phase of a trial. During the second phase of a trial the landmarks
reappeared and the participant was required to point to the landmark that the
character was hiding behind; the position of the array could be translated
and rotated between phases of each trial. During half the trials the
geometrical configuration of the three landmarks could be used to locate the
correct hiding location of the character; during the other trials only the
features of each landmark could be used to locate the hidden character. All
48 of the adult participants readily used both geometry and feature cues, the
data from the children will be forthcoming. |
Goto |
Kazuhiro Goto, Alan C. Kamil,
Alan B. Bond (University of Nebraska) |
Interactions
between associative and sequential priming |
Facilitative
effects of associative and sequential priming have often been reported in the
literature, but little is known about their relationship. In this study, we
examined how these two types of priming might interact. Four groups of five blue jays each were
trained to search for two types of cryptic moth. At the beginning of each trial, green
horizontal lines or red vertical lines were given as primes; pecks to the
prime initiated the trial. The four
conditions were: 1) Only primes predicted target types (associative priming),
2) only trial sequence (i.e. AAAA...BBBB... etc.) predicted target types
(sequential priming), 3) both primes and trial sequence predicted target
types (combinational priming) or 4) Neither primes nor sequence predicted
target types (control). The effects of
the different combinations of associative and sequential priming will allow
assessment of the nature of any interactions between the two types of priming
procedure. |
Guilhardi |
Paulo Guilhardi (Brown
University) |
Predicting
Random Interval Choice on Basis of the Behavior on the Alternatives |
The
goal was to determine whether choice behavior between random interval
alternatives could be predicted from the summation of behavior on each of the
alternatives. Twenty-four rats were trained with two stimuli, each with a
mean duration of 120s that could be presented simultaneously or successively.
With probability 0.5, food was delivered at a random interval of 15s during
one stimulus and at a random interval of 60s during the other stimulus. Half
of the rats were trained with a single lever, and the other half were trained
with different levers for the two intervals (choice procedure). The same
simple quantitative rule based on the summation of the alternatives that
accounted for behavior in a previous experiment with fixed interval
alternatives, accounted for the choice performance. These results suggest
that common principles determine behavior on a wide range of procedures such
as fixed and random interval, peak, and choice. |
Hampton |
Robert R. Hampton (Emery
University) |
Does
Test-Related Frustration Indicate Metamemory in a Rhesus Monkey (Macaca
mulatta)? |
Humans
can predict with some accuracy whether or not they know the correct answer to
a question before responding. In some cases the capacity to make such
predictions depends on metamemory, the ability to make introspective
judgments about memory states. In this unplanned retrospective analysis of
video taped behavior we asked whether a monkey’s apparent frustration
predicted his accuracy in a matching-to-sample task on a trial by trial
basis. The monkey was likely to make his test response by aggressively
slapping the computer touch screen on error trials, whereas he generally
touched the screen more gently when selecting a response on correct trials.
This difference in behavior, which occurred before the monkey received
feedback on the accuracy of his choice, suggests that he knew whether or not
he remembered the correct response. |
Supported
by the NIMH-IRP. |
Hattori |
Yuko Hattori.,Hika
Kuroshima.,& Kazuo Fujita.(Kyoto University) |
Pointing
and sensitivity to the human attentional states in tufted capuchin monkeys
(Cebus apella) |
Previous
reports suggest that nonhuman primates can use pointing gestures differentially for obtaining food
based on the experimenter’s directions of body and face but not on eyes,
although neurophysiological studies suggest that they have a sensitivity to
the state of eyes. We investigated whether a New World monkey species, tufted
capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), respond differentially to several human
states of the face and eyes for obtaining food, by analyzing both frequency
of pointing gestures and duration of gaze toward the experimenter. The
results showed that the monkeys looked at the experimenter differentially
according to the attentional states, but did not point differentially. This
suggests that the previous failure for nonhumans to differentially respond
based on eyes may not be due to their lack of sensitivity to eyes but to the
lack of communicative function in their pointing gestures to request food
from humans. |
Hernández |
Michelle Hernández & Jeffrey
S. Katz (Auburn University) |
Pigeons
Learn the Function of Directional Arrows in a Touch Screen Monitor Task |
Three
pigeons learned to use directional arrows (left, right, down, up) to move a
target to different goal locations on a touch screen monitor. In a series of
experiments the complexity of the task was manipulated by increasing the
number of directional arrows, distance to the goal, location of the goal, and
position of a barrier. Performance was assessed by the number of responses to
reach the goal. The pigeons often used the shortest path to the goal, but
were not always optimal. In trials with two arrows (left, right), performance
decreased as the distance between the target and goal increased, but was
influenced by the position of the barrier. In trials with four arrows,
performance was most disrupted when the barrier was placed between the target
and goal but improved as the distance between the target and goal
increased. |
Himmanen |
Sharon Himmanen (Lehman
College/CUNY) & Karyl Swartz (Great Ape Trust of Iowa and Lehman
College/CUNY) |
A
Spatial Strategy May Facilitate Recognition Memory |
Strategies
for executing lists were examined in monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and orangutans
(Pongo pygmaeus spp.). List items
were presented individually on a touch-sensitive video monitor, followed by
the simultaneous presentation of those items as well as distractor
items. Subjects were rewarded for
correctly touching all list items, in any order, without touching a
distractor item. Two orangutans and
one monkey developed a right-to-left pattern of responding, which emerged as
the cognitive demands of the task increased (during 5-item lists for
orangutans and 3-item lists for the monkey).
This simple spatial strategy decreased cognitive load by reducing
working-memory demands associated with remembering previously searched
locations. |
Hultsch |
Henrike Hultsch (Freie
Universität Berlin) |
Imitation
and Sequencing of Songs by Nightingales Follows Contextual Cues at the Time
of Auditory Acquisition |
Hand-reared
juvenile male Nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos) were exposed to song
stimuli during temporally distributed tutoring sessions under two regimes.
Under the Strobe regime (SR) strobe lights were paired to the playback of
strings of master songs, while under the Control regime (CR) there were no
additional cues during exposure to master songs. Subjects experienced a
single song string under one of the two regimes per day in the following
order: CR1-> CR2-> SR3-> CR4-> SR5-> SR6-> CR7-> SR8.
Singing recorded 11 months later revealed that males had preferentially
acquired SR songs. In addition, they sequentially clustered imitations of
song-types from either regime (eg. SR3-> SR8 or CR2-> CR4) regardless
of sequencing at the time of tutoring. The findings suggest that intermodal
stimulus pairing affects stimulus selection and memorisation through
attentional mechanisms. Performance characteristics, in addition, suggest
that birds stored song stimuli as a set reflecting contextual cues from the
tutoring sessions. |
Inagaki |
Tomoko Inagaki(CUNY Graduate
Center, Hunter C.), Josephine Choe(Fordham Uiversity) |
Relative
judgments of quantitative information by pigeons using visual display |
What
do pigeons learn when quantitative information is represented as different
numbers of discrete items or as parts of a continuous area? Two groups of 3 pigeons each, were tested
with a row of 5 rectangles (discrete group) or a horizontal bar (continuous
group). The proportion of red to green elements or area of the bar varied
from all red to all green in 5 steps. Under both conditions, the proportion
of choices correct for red (or green) increased as the number or proportion
of red (or green) increased. These functions were virtually unchanged when
the arrangement of the colors was changed and when the rectangles were
changed to disks. However, when the discrete group was tested with the
horizontal bar and the continuous group was tested with the 5 rectangles, two
birds, one from each group, failed to respond differentially to the stimuli.
These data suggest that different mechanisms may be involved when
quantitative information is presented in discrete or in continuous form. |
Johnson |
P. Taylor Johnson, Angie C.
Koban, & Robert G. Cook (Tufts University) |
Categorical
Same Different Discrimination in the Pigeon (Columba livia) |
The
ability of pigeons to learn a categorical same different task was
investigated by presenting subjects with 4 items: three distractors and one
target item to which the pigeon had to locate and respond. The three distractor items consisted of
different pictures of one category (i.e. 3 different pictures of cats), while
the fourth item was that of a different category (i.e. car). An identity-based same different task was
also examined for control conditions.
This condition consisted of three distractor items that were identical
such that the pigeon simply had to differentiate the odd item. Acquisition of the task (Identity >
Categorical) and transfer data will be discussed as well as possible
implications for the origins of analogical thinking. |
Kacelnik |
Alex Kacelnik, Alex Weir,
Christian Rutz & Ben Kenward (University of Oxford) |
Tool
manufacture by naïve hand-reared New Caledonian Crows |
New
Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) are the most prolific avian
tool-users, and regional variation in the shape of their tools may be the
result of cumulative cultural evolution — a phenomenon considered to be a
hallmark of human culture. We show that hand-raised juvenile New Caledonian
crows spontaneously manufacture and use tools, without any contact with
adults of their species or any prior demonstration by humans. Our finding
highlights the importance of developmental studies for producing informed
models of cultural transmission in this species, and in animals in general.
The talk will be illustrated with relevant video clips. |
Kamil |
Alan B. Bond and Alan C. Kamil
(University of Nebraska, Lincoln) |
Effects
of Background Heterogeneity on the Evolution of Cryptic Prey |
In this
experiment, we used our virtual ecology paradigm to evaluate the effects of
background heterogeneity on the evolution of polymorphism. In nthis paradigm, blue jays hunt for
cryptic prey whose appearance is controlled by a virtual genome. Moths that escape detection are more likely
to breed than those which are detected, resulting in the evolution of the
appearance of the moths. In this
experiment, jays hunted moths on three different backgrounds, which varied in
heterogeneity. Results clearly show
that variation in background affects the evolution of prey, especially the
evolution of phenotypic diversity. |
Katz |
Jeffrey S. Katz, Kent D. Bodily,
Michelle Hernandez, & Bradley R. Sturz (Auburn University), & Anthony
A. Wright (The University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston) |
Comparing
Concept Learning in Matching-to-Sample and Same/Different by Pigeons |
Abstract
concept learning in matching-to-sample and same/different tasks was compared
using a set-size expansion procedure. In separate groups (matching-to-sample,
same/different), training began with a small set size and was systematically
doubled until a high level of concept learning was attained. To measure
concept learning, transfer tests with novel stimuli were conducted after each
set size increase. Transfer performance increased with set size.
Matching-to-sample and same/different concept learning were compared by
plotting transfer performance at each set size by the total number of display
configurations with the training stimuli. Under the conditions of these
experiments, the set-size functions were similar for both tasks. The
processes of abstract concept learning in these two tasks are discussed in
terms of their similar functions relating concept learning to set size and
the high level of concept learning ultimately achieved by the subjects in
both these tasks. |
Keen |
Richard Keen (Converse College)
& Russell Church (Brown University) |
Simultaneous
Timing: Combination Rules |
The
purpose of this study was to determine how multiple timing cues affect timing
behavior. During training, rats were
reinforced for the first head entry response after 120 s following the
previous food. Four groups differed in
when a 1 s stimulus was presented: 0, 30, 60, or 90 s following the previous
food. During testing, four types of
trials were presented: 1) normal trials were those in which food and a
stimulus were presented, 2) food only trials, 3) signal only trials, and 4)
empty trials, neither food nor stimuli were presented. Normal trials were presented on the odd
trials and the four trial types had an equal probability of occurring on the
even trials. Results showed that both
the food-to-food and signal-to-food intervals were learned. Response patterning is well characterized
by the Packet Theory of Timing. |
Kelly |
Debbie M. Kelly & Alan C.
Kamil (University of Nebraska, Lincoln) |
Effects
of Landmark Configuration on Search Accuracy by Clark's nutcrackers |
Several
experiments have shown that Clark's nutcrackers use objects surrounding the
cache site as landmarks for successful cache retrieval. The Multiple Bearings Hypothesis (Kamil
& Cheng, 2001) predicts that the specific configuration of available
landmarks will strongly influence search accuracy. The purpose of our experiment was to test
these predictions by measuring the effect of landmark configuration on search
accuracy on the estimation of distance and direction. Four groups of
nutcrackers were trained to locate a hidden goal at a fixed distance (i.e.,
90 cm) from either one, two or three available landmarks. The configuration of the landmarks differed
between the groups. Upon completion of
training, the groups were given non-reinforced tests in which one of the
landmarks was shifted by 30 cm in one of four directions (i.e., north, south,
east or west). The results confirm
several predictions of the multiple-bearings hypothesis, but also suggest
that the hypothesis provides an incomplete account of how landmarks are used
to re-locate positions in space. |
Klein |
Emily D. Klein, Dustin J.
Stairs, & Michael T. Bardo (University of Kentucky) |
Acquisition,
Extinction and Reinstatement of Operant Responding in Differentially Reared
Rats |
The
current study examined the effect of differential rearing on acquisition,
extinction, and reinstatement of responding for a sucrose reward. Rats reared
in either an enriched (EC) or impoverished (IC) environment were trained to
lever press for sucrose on an FR 1 schedule of reinforcement. Rats were then
tested for response persistence during extinction. A “prime” of 1 or 10
sucrose pellets (counterbalanced for order of presentation) was given at the
beginning of extinction sessions 11 and 14. During acquisition, EC rats made
significantly more lever presses than IC rats. However, during extinction,
this trend was reversed, with IC rats making significantly more lever presses
than EC rats. The 10-pellet prime increased lever pressing significantly more
than the 1-pellet prime in EC, but not IC, rats. The results suggest that EC
rats are more sensitive to changes in response contingencies than IC rats. |
Koban |
Koban, A.C., Beale, K. &
Cook, R.G. (Tufts University) |
Motion
categorization by Pigeons |
Pigeons
were tested in a go/no-go task requiring the classification of 40 different
video objects based on their rate of rotation (fast versus slow) around their
central axis. After acquisition, pigeons transferred this rate discrimination
to novel objects and novel directions of rotation. The nature of the
discrimination was further examined by varying the speed of rotation. The
results suggest that pigeons can form a motion-based concept based on the
rate of object motion. Implications for the development of motion and action
categories and the use of video stimuli to portray motion will be discussed. |
Kuczaj |
Stan Kuczaj (University of
Southern Mississippi), Leslie Popiel, Chris White, & Chris Bellows
(SeaWorld San Antonio) |
Beluga
(Delphinapterus leucas) Use of Echolocation to Perceive Actions |
Recent
evidence suggests that dolphins can relate visual and acoustic information
about objects, but nothing is known about the ability of cetaceans to use
information about actions learned in one perceptual system (e.g., vision)
with information subsequently received in another perceptual system (e.g.,
echolocation). We asked a female
beluga to respond to underwater gestures that she could not see, but could
use echolocation to perceive. The
gestures were ones to which the beluga had earlier been trained to respond
when presented visually above the surface of the water. She proved adept at translating what she
had learned visually about these actions to a new modality (echolocation),
providing further support for the notion that cetaceans’ representations of
their world may be abstract rather than modality specific. |
Kundey |
Shannon M.A. Kundey & Laurie
R. Santos (Yale University) |
Can
capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use non-arbitrary relationships to make
inferences about the world? |
Call
(2004) examined whether après, like humans, use non-arbitrary cues to make
inferences about the world. He baited one of two cups and provided either
visual or auditory information about the baiting. Auditory cues included either shaking
(non-arbitrary) or tapping (arbitrary) the cup. Great apes spontaneously use non-arbitrary
relationships to make inferences in this task. Based on these findings, we questioned if a
monkey species, the capuchin, could similarly use non-arbitrary cues in this
task. Subjects selected the baited box
when given visual information but performed at chance when given no
information. When presented with auditory information, only subjects given
the non-arbitrary cue correctly chose the baited box. Experiment 2 presented subjects with
information about only 1 box. Subjects
successfully used visual information but succeeded only when provided
auditory information about the baited box. |
Kuroshima |
Hika Kuroshima, Yuko Hattori,
Kazuo Fujita(Department of Psychology Kyoto University, JSPS) |
Spontaneous
food exchange with human caretakers by a capuchin monkey: Recognition of the
opportunity and the rates of exchange |
One
male tufted capuchin monkey spontaneously began to hand his food to
caretakers. We investigated his rule of this exchange. In experiment 1, he
was given opportunities to exchange his chows with various foods from the
caretaker. As the results, he came to hand his chow only when the
experimenter showed her food. When an unfamiliar person worked as the
experimenter, the monkey didn't apply this acquired rule. In experiment 2, we
investigated how the subject rated the value of each food. We found that he
had stable values for each kind of foods. In experiment 3, the food that the
monkey kept was changed. When he kept his favorite food, he exchanged at
lower rates than the original situation. These results indicate that he
recognized the opportunity to exchange and spontaneously attributed
differential values to each food according to the situation. |
Lazareva |
Olga F. Lazareva, Shaun P.
Vecera, and Edward A. Wasserman (University of Iowa) |
Figure-Ground
Assignment in Pigeons |
We
trained four pigeons to discriminate whether a target spot appeared on a
colored figural shape or on a differently colored background. The birds first had to peck the
target. The birds then had to report
the location of the target—on the figure or on the background. We recorded three dependent measures: target detection time, choice response
time, and choice accuracy. A figural
advantage was seen in all three dependent measures: The birds were faster to detect the target,
to report its location, and to learn the correct response on figure trials
than on background trials. This
figural advantage emerged in acquisition and it was retained after prolonged
training. However, when four other
birds were required to detect and peck the target without making a choice
report, no figural advantage appeared in target detection time. Thus, pigeons’ attention to foreground
figures may be affected by task demands. |
Lea |
Stephen E. G. Lea (University of
Exeter), Kazuhiro Goto (University of Nebraska-Lincoln) and Andy J. Wills
(University of Exeter) |
Theoretical
analysis of concept learning |
In
concept discrimination learning tasks, multiple instances of defined positive
and negative stimulus categories are presented. Acquisition is usually
measured in terms of overall performance at discriminating the categories. In this paper, we present
stimulus-by-stimulus analyses of acquisition of such discriminations in both
birds and humans. The analyses show
that (i) Learning about individual stimuli can be well represented by a
process involving abrupt transitions between a small number of states, but
(ii) the transition points for different stimuli are not independent. |
Leising |
Kenneth J. Leising (University
of California, Los Angeles), Kosuke Sawa (Japan Society for the Promotion of
Science, Nagoya University; University of California, Los Angeles), and Aaron
P. Blaisdell (University of California, Los Angeles) |
Extinction
and spontaneous recovery of spatial information using a touchscreen task with
pigeons |
Extinction
and spontaneous recovery of spatial information was investigated using a
landmark-based spatial-search task on a touchscreen in pigeons. Four visual
landmarks were separately established as signals to a hidden goal location on
the screen. The goal was located above Landmarks (LMs) A and C and below LMs
B and D. After conditioning, LMs A and B were extinguished. Responding to LMs
A and C was assessed on subsequent tests two days following extinction, while
LMs B and D were tested fourteen days after extinction. Finally, all LMs were
tested 42 days after extinction treatment. Extinction of spatial control over
search by a visual landmark was observed after two days, while spontaneous
recovery was observed at longer delays. |
Lewis |
Jody L. Lewis & Alan C.
Kamil (University of Nebraska -Lincoln) |
Changing
the landmark array decreases proactive interference in Clark’s nutcrackers,
Nucifraga columbiana |
Previously,
we have demonstrated that Clark’s nutcrackers are susceptible to proactive
interference when given a serial spatial memory task. That is, nutcrackers make more errors when
given two lists a day versus one. In
this experiment, the effects of proactive interference were alleviated by
changing the landmark array between list one and list two. |
Locurto |
Chuck Locurto (College of the
Holy Cross) |
Mice
learn a win-shift but not a win-stay contingency under water escape
motivation |
In
foraging situations animals generally prefer win-shift to win-stay learning,
that is, they learn more quickly to visit new locations compared to returning
to already-visited locations to obtain food. This preference is typically
explained by appeal to some form of optimal foraging. The complementary
preference under aversive motivation has been less well studied although
extant interpretations appeal to the common sense idea that in escape
situations animals would prefer to return to a formerly safe location rather
than visit a new location, that is, they would prefer win-stay to win-shift
learning. Three experiments using mice
examined the acquisition of either a win-shift or a win-stay contingency
under water escape motivation. Results
were contrary to this common sense notion. Mice uniformly preferred win-shift
to win-stay learning to the extent that while win-shift learning was rapidly
acquired, few subjects exceeded chance performance even after extended
exposure to a win-stay contingency. |
Lyn |
Heidi Lyn (Wildlife Conservation
Society) & Diana Reiss (Wildlife Conservation Society and Columbia
University) |
Similarities
in Use and Comprehension of Symbol Systems in Apes and Dolphins: Analogies in
Cognition |
Studies
of symbolic competency in nonhumans have been well documented. Many
researchers have focused on nonhuman primates under the assumption that human
symbol use is primarily biological and therefore similar abilities would most
likely be found in our closest evolutionary relatives. However, published and
new data from two long-term studies with bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), one investigating the
learning and productive use of an interactive keyboard (Reiss and McCowan,
1993) and one investigating the comprehension of artificial symbol systems
(Herman et al., 1984, 1993), suggest representational and conceptual
abilities which are strikingly similar to both humans and apes. New analyses
reported here show parallels in acquisition of associations, behavioral
concordance, rule learning, and predominance of error types between dolphins
and great apes that support the hypothesis that the abilities of primates and
dolphins may result from analogous cognitive evolution. |
MacInnis |
Mika L.M. MacInnis (Brown
University) |
The
Effect of Stimulus State and Probability on Behavior |
The
goal was to determine how the type of stimulus (a brief pulse or a filled
interval of white noise) and probability of stimulus presentation interact to
influence behavior. Each of 42 rats
was trained on one of seven instrumental appetitive head entry procedures, in
which food was available every 120 s and a stimulus was presented 30 s prior
to the next food delivery (either a brief pulse, or a filled 30-s
interval). The stimulus was presented
with a probability of 0 (where the stimulus was never presented), 0.33, 0.67,
or 1 (either the brief pulse or the filled interval). The extent of use of the time cue provided
by the stimulus was inversely related to the probability of stimulus
delivery. The results were modeled
using a Packet Theory of Timing, with a linear averaging rule to combine the
differential influences of the stimulus and food cues. |
Makecha |
Radhika Makecha & Stan
Kuczaj (University of Southern Mississippi) |
Changes in a Dolphin's Preferential Play Partners |
Recent
work has suggested that young dolphins prefer to play with other young
dolphins rather than adults (including their mothers). We expand on this work by investigating
changes in a young female dolphin’s preferential play partners during the
first four years of her life.
Initially, the dolphin played most with an older calf (during this
period, the older calf was the only other calf in the tank). As new calves
were born, she played more with the new calves and less with her once almost
constant companion. In addition, her
early play consisted mainly of object play and social play, but her later
play incorporated more allomaternal behaviors, suggesting that the dolphin
began to use play to acquire and perfect maternal behaviors as well as to
enrich her life. |
Miller |
Noam Miller & Sara J.
Shettleworth (University of Toronto) |
What is
Geometry and When is it Used? |
It is
now well established that rats, chicks, goldfish, monkeys, and humans can
learn to use the overall geometric shape of an enclosure to find a goal. In
such studies subjects are usually deprived of information about their
location in the world outside the enclosure. We asked whether idiothetic cues
(path integration) and landmarks outside the enclosure affect the learning of
geometric information. Rats were trained with both geometric and either
idiothetic or extra-enclosure landmark information available and then tested
on whether they learned both and which they used in case of conflict. We also
tested the hypothesis that geometric information is encoded as the principal
axes of a space by training rats in an enclosure of one shape and then deforming
it in ways that maintained some of the sides and angles while changing their
relationship to the long or short axes of the enclosure. |
Miller |
Tom Beckers (University of
Leuven), Ralph R. Miller (SUNY-Binghamton), Jan De Houwer (University of
Ghent), and Kouji Urushihara (SUNY-Binghamton) |
Reasoning Rats: Inference-like
Processing of Information Concerning Effect Additivity and Ceilings |
According
to contemporary associative learning theories, forward blocking arises
directly from a hard-wired basic learning algorithm that governs the
acquisition and expression of associations. Seemingly contrary to this view,
recent research has yielded evidence of inference-like information processing
in humans confronted with both Pavlovian and causal assessment tasks. Here we demonstrate that blocking in rats
is also seemingly sensitive to constraints of logical causal inference, such
as violation of causal additivity and ceiling considerations. This argues for
similar information processing in rats and humans and suggests that complex
cognitive processes akin to causal inferential reasoning are involved in animal
conditioning phenomena commonly attributed to the operation of basic
associative processes. |
Nagasaka |
Yasuo Nagasaka (University of
Iowa), Koji Hori, & Yoshihisa Osada (Rikkyo University) |
Recognition
of depth relationship between surfaces unveils amodal completion in pigeons |
Some
studies have obtained evidence that various animals can recognize partly
occluded objects in the same manner as humans. However, there is no positive
evidence for amodal completion in pigeons. We assume the reason for such
negative results lies in the tasks used in prior studies, which requires the
subject to discriminate the object itself. In our study, therefore, we
investigated the perception of amodal completion together with perceptual
transparency in pigeons by using a task that probes the depth relationship
between objects. If subjects can discriminate the depth relationship between
surfaces, then such evidence would suggest that the observer completes the
occluded object. Our results indicate that the pigeons can discriminate the
depth relationship between opaque and transparent surfaces, suggesting that
pigeons are also capable of amodal completion. |
Neiworth |
Julie Neiworth, Alison Lewis,
& Maren Sonstegard (Carleton College) |
Assessment of Number by Tamarins |
Two
different methods were employed to determine the abilities of a new world
monkey species, cotton top tamarins, to assess number. In a visual
simultaneous assessment task, monkeys were presented with 2 3 X 3 matrices
containing 2 and 4 sweetened cereal O’s placed in unique patterns, and were
trained to select the matrix containing “4”. They were then tested with
different novel amounts compared with 4 (e.g., 1 vs. 4, and 4 vs. 6) to
determine if they had learned the discrimination by subitizing, or were using
a numerosity assessment of “greater than” to make their selection. In a
visual successive task, tamarins observed the sequential dropping of
sweetened cereal into 2 different inverted cups (of the amounts 3 vs. 6 and 0
vs. 6) and were allowed to tip over a single cup to obtain rewards. The
number “6” was rewarded, and others were not. Following correct performance,
the monkeys were tested with various “addition” problems in which the same
total amounts were tested, but the quantities were presented as 2 different
dropping visits (e.g., 1+2 vs. 3+3 or 2+1 vs. 5+1). Their behaviors indicated
the kinds of assessment of which they are capable, ranging from numerosity,
to subitizing, to numeric abstractions such as summation. |
Otovic |
Pete Otovic, Michael Rogers
& Sarah Partan (University of South Florida) |
Salience
of Multi-Sensory Stimuli in Pigeon Courtship Sequences (Columba livia) |
By
analyzing courtship behavior sequences of male and female pigeons, we
investigated the effects of certain male stimuli, such as the bow and coo, on
the female pigeon’s behavior. In
addition we investigated the degree of redundance observed in auditory and
visual components of the male’s behavior.
We tested six female pigeons using video playback methodology and lag
sequential data analysis. The data indicated that the female was more likely
to walk in a circle after the male’s bow and coo than during her baseline
behavior. We also found that the
combination of auditory and visual stimuli, in the male bow-coo behavior,
elicited more frequent female walk-circle behavior than either auditory or
visual conditions alone. We conclude
that video playbacks are effective tools for eliciting responses in female
pigeons and conducting sequential analyses, and that the multisensory audio
and visual signal from the male carries an enhanced message. |
Patton |
Tadd B. Patton, Gabrielle M.
Kellner, & Toru Shimizu (University of South Florida) |
Is
there something about her eyes? Significance of the Head Region on Preference
Behaviors in Pigeons (Columba livia) |
Laboratory
studies have shown that pigeons can recognize conspecifics and select
potential mates even when only visual information is available. Specifically, these studies indicate that
information in the head and/or neck is important for conspecific
recognition. However, it is still
unclear whether specific features of the face (e.g., eyes, beak) are more
important than others (e.g., shape of the head). In order to answer this question, the
current study measured male pigeons’ preference for various photographs of
female pigeons. Some of the
photographic images were modified so that specific features were exaggerated
or reduced (e.g., large or small eyes).
In other photographs, configuration of features was altered (e.g.,
removal of high frequency information).
The results will be discussed in relation to the perceptual and
cognitive strategies that pigeons use for conspecific recognition. |
Rowan |
James D. Rowan, Amanda R.
Willey, Eric P. Nolley, and Brian M. Kelley (Bridgewater College) |
Adolescence
exposure to Fluoxetine (Prozac) impairs adult serial-pattern learning in
rats. |
This
experiment examines the effects of early exposure to Fluoxetine (Prozac) on
adult higher cognitive function.
Weanling rats were injected 5 days a week for 5 weeks with equivalent
volumes of 4mg/kg or saline based on body weight. After 5 weeks off, all
subjects were trained on a violation trill pattern (123 234 345 456 567 678
781 818) for 28 days receiving 5 patterns a day. The Prozac exposed group
learned the pattern at approximately the same rate as the saline group but
the pattern of errors for the two groups greatly differed. Rats in the Prozac group made more errors
in the violation chunk. Also, the
Prozac group produced more errors at chunk boundaries and less errors within
chunks when compared to the control group. |
Santi |
Angelo Santi, Jennifer Lellwitz,
and Stephen Gagne (Wilfrid Laurier University) |
Pigeons’ Memory for Sequences of Light Flashes: Control
by Temporal Rather than Numerical Features |
The
question of whether pigeons use the same or different processes to
discriminate time and number has been of continuing interest to Bill Roberts.
Bill’s initial research on memory for number suggested that similar processes
were involved, but his more recent work provides evidence of different
processes. Studies of memory for light flash sequences in our lab involved
training pigeons match two sets of sample sequences each consisting of two
light flashes in 4s and four light flashes in 4s. The duration of a flash was
varied so that neither individual flash duration nor total flash duration
could serve as a reliable cue for responding to all of the sample sequences
(i.e., 2f x 400 ms, 4f x 200 ms, 2f x 800 ms, and 4f x 400 ms). Data will be
presented which suggests that several temporal features of these light flash
sequences controlled responding rather than number. |
Schmajuk |
Nestor Schmajuk and Jose
Larrauri (Duke University) |
A neural network model that describes super latent
inhiition |
It was
recently demonstrated that a delay interposed between conditioning and
testing results in an increased latent inhibition effect, a phenomenon termed
super-latent inhibition. We show that an existing neural network model of
classical conditioning (Schmajuk, Lam, and Gray, 1996), which includes an
attentional mechanism that controls both storage and retrieval of
associations, is able to describe many of the properties of super-latent
inhibition. |
Schneider |
Susan M. Schneider and Robert
Lickliter (Florida International University) |
Auditory Generalization
Gradients in Bobwhite Quail Chicks |
Bobwhite
quail chicks respond differentially to a variety of species-typical calls and
can discriminate small changes in the repetition rate of a maternal
call. To further explore their
auditory sensitivity, newly hatched chicks were trained nondifferentially to
peck a touchscreen on a variable ratio heat reinforcement schedule while a
beep was repeated at an intermediate rate; initial autoshaping with this
training stimulus facilitated quick learning.
We obtained typical individual generalization gradients over seven
beep rates in extinction from chicks at 3 to 5 days of age. Sometimes mirror-image inhibitory gradients
occurred. Stimuli associated with
aversives produce inhibitory gradients, so these chicks may be reporting
their hedonic states. Our findings
with quail neonates extend research with older populations suggesting that
generalization operates similarly across reinforcers and aversives. Results will be compared with those from
chicks trained on the same schedule but without the auditory stimulus, and
from chicks trained differentially. |
Schwartz |
Bennett L. Schwartz (Florida
International University), Megan L. Hoffman (Georgia State University),
Melinda Allen (Florida International University), Jonathan D. Lane (Florida
International University), & Heather Cherry (Miami-Dade College) |
Working memory in Lar Gibbons (Hylobates lar) |
Working
memory refers to an animal’s ability to remember stimuli or events presented
during a single trial. We used a
delayed-response task and a delayed match-to-sample task (DMTS) to test the
working memory of two lar gibbons (Hylobates Lar). In our first experiment, a spatial
delayed-response task, the gibbons were required to remember the location of
hidden grapes. The male gibbon performed
above chance at retention intervals that averaged over 90 seconds. In our next two experiments, the female
gibbon showed above-chance performance at remembering the placement of a
grape in one of three cups placed directly in front of the gibbon. In a fourth experiment, the female gibbon performed above chance on
an object-identity DMTS with a retention interval averaging 11 seconds. In all four experiments, with little prior
training, at least one gibbon performed at above-chance levels, suggesting the ability to represent past
events in working memory. |
Shettleworth |
What-Where-When Memory in
Pigeons |
Shannon
I. Skov-Rackette, Noam Miller, and Sara J. Shettleworth (University of
Toronto) |
An
animal that encodes the identity, location, and time of an event should
respond correctly to any of the three questions, “what was it?”, “where was
it?”, or “when was it?” when it cannot anticipate which will be asked. We
developed a novel approach to testing “episodic-like” memory based on this
assumption. Pigeons were trained to match the identity of a 3-s sample on a
touchscreen, to match its location, and to report on the length of the
retention interval (2s or 6s). The three tasks were trained in separate,
interleaved, sessions until the birds reached a criterion of 80% correct on
all of them at both RIs. Then blocks
of “what”, ‘when”, and “where” trials were intermixed within each session.
Performance on “when”, but not “what” or “where” initially decreased
dramatically in these sessions, suggesting that the birds do not
simultaneously encode all three features. |
Singer |
Rebecca A. Singer & Thomas
R. Zentall (University of Kentucky) |
Formation of Cognitive Maps in Rats |
We
examined the ability of rats to develop and use cognitive maps. Rats were
trained to retrieve food rewards from two of the three goal boxes of a
three-arm maze. Each arm, but not the goal box, was uniquely textured, which
allowed rats to orient themselves within the maze. On test trials, rats were
allowed to choose between two novel pathways from the center goal box, one of
which led to the goal box that had been baited in training and the other
which led to the goal box that had been empty. Rats chose the novel shortcut
that led to the arm that had been baited in training significantly more than
the novel shortcut that led to the arm that had not been baited. These
results suggest that rats were able to navigate along a novel path using an
internal map of their environment. |
Stahlman |
W. David Stahlman (University of
California, Los Angeles), Seth Roberts (University of California, Berkeley),
and Aaron P. Blaisdell (University of California, Los Angeles) |
Response
Variability is Inversely Related to Probability of Reward: A Demonstration in
Instrumental Screen Pecking in Pigeons |
Gharib,
Gade, & Roberts (2004) have shown that variability in the duration of bar
pressing behavior in rats increases as a function of decreasing reward
probability signaled by the discriminative stimulus. We report a replication of this result in
pigeons pecking a visual stimulus on a touchscreen. On a given trial, a single red or blue
circle was presented on the touchscreen.
Pigeons were required to peck at the stimulus to proceed to the next
trial. One stimulus (e.g., red) was
always followed by reinforcement, while the other (e.g., blue) was reinforced
only once in every four or eight trials.
Most subjects showed greater spatial variability of pecks on trials
with the low-probability than with the high-probability stimulus, consistent
with the findings of Gharib et al.
However, we also report some surprising individual differences and
counterintuitive observations. |
Stevens |
Jeffrey R. Stevens, Alexandra G.
Rosati, and Marc D. Hauser (Harvard University) |
The
value of space and time for two New World monkeys |
Trading
off small, immediate rewards for large, delayed rewards is a common dilemma
facing all animal species. Often
animals discount the future, exhibiting a preference for immediate rewards. Animals must discount not only over time,
but also over space—closer rewards should be more valuable than distant
rewards. These types of choices that
require travel over a distance often reflect more naturalistic foraging
techniques than strict temporal discounting tasks. We tested two primate species—common
marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) and cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)—in
a spatial discounting task in which subjects chose between small, close
rewards and large, distant rewards.
Tamarins traveled farther distances than marmosets contradicting
previous findings in the temporal discounting context. These findings, however, match the foraging
and daily travel patterns of both marmosets and tamarins, suggesting that the
species’ ecology can play an important role in shaping decision making. |
Sturdy |
Tiffany T.-Y. Lee, Isabelle
Charrier, Laurie L. Bloomfield, Christopher B. Sturdy (University of
Alberta), & Ronald G. Weisman (Queen’s University) |
Frequency
range discriminations in chickadees and finches |
The
pitch, or frequency, of many songbird vocalizations plays an integral role in
species recognition. Here we trained male and female black-capped and
mountain chickadees, along with male zebra finches, to sort frequencies into
contiguous rewarded and non-rewarded ranges. Following discrimination
training we conducted a transfer test to determine whether birds categorized
tones by range or instead memorized individual tones. In line with previous
studies, finches performed at a high level and outperformed both species of
chickadees on some performance measures. In contrast to prior results with
zebra finches, male and female chickadees did not differ in their absolute
pitch perception abilities. Results of the transfer test suggest that all
three species of songbirds categorized rather than memorized novel stimuli.
The possible causes of the relatively poor performance of chickadees compared
to zebra finches and potential proximate mechanisms underlying the lack of
sex differences in chickadees will be discussed. |
Sturz |
Sturz, B. R. & Bodily, K. D.
(Auburn University) |
Virtual
open-field: Evidence against integration of spatial maps in humans |
In a
previous study, human participants navigated a virtual, open-field analogue
of Blaisdell & Cook (2005).
Results were consistent with those found in pigeons. Additional analyses suggested an
accumulation of non-reinforced choice responses (not integration) was
responsible for changes in responding across tests. A follow-up study eliminated the critical
stimulus for integration in pigeons (i.e., reintroduction of Phase1 trials
during Test 2). Phase 1 and Phase 2
training were identical to the previous study, but during transfer, Phase 2
trials were substituted for Phase 1 trials.
Thus, testing blocks were comprised of Phase 2, Phase 2, and red L
alone. The results were not different
from those obtained when Phase 1 “reminder” trials were included for both
humans and pigeons. Overall, the
findings provided evidence that changes in cup choices were not due to an
integration of spatial maps. |
Subiaul |
Francys Subiaul (University of
Louisiana, Lafayette) & Herbert S. Terrace (Columbia University) |
Is Learning from Observation as Good as Learning from
Triral and Error? |
Do
we learn best from individual experience? Or, is imitation learning just as
good? Research from the cognitive neurosciences suggests that the mechanisms
through which observation contributes to learning a motor skill are very
similar to the mechanisms underlying individual trial-and-error learning.
Here, we re-analyzed data collected from two adult rhesus macaques tested on
a cognitive imitation study (i.e., Subiaul et al., 2004) to test whether
learning by cognitive imitation was as robust as learning resulting from
individual trial and error experience. Our data suggests that among rhesus
macaques ordinal knowledge acquired by cognitive imitation doesn’t
significantly differ from ordinal knowledge that results from individual
trial and error [Horatio: F(4,98) = .84; Oberon: F(4,116) = .20, One-Way
ANOVA]. These results support the hypothesis that the mechanisms mediating
learning by trial and error are the same as the mechanisms mediating learning
by imitation. |
Suits |
William T. Suits, Rebecca Doak
& Janice N. Steirn (Georgia Southern University) |
Transitive
Inference with Commonly Coded Stimuli |
This
experiment examines whether stimuli
that are commonly coded through association with similar outcomes will be
treated differently when used in a transitive inference (TI) task than
stimuli not commonly coded. A TI task was used in which stimuli were
presented to the pigeons as pairs (A+B-, B+C-, C+D-, D+E-), and a single pair
of stimuli were presented as a test (BD).
For the Within Common Coding Group, the outcomes were arranged so that
B and D both received the same outcome (when positive in their respective
pairs). For the Between Common Coding Group, the outcomes were arranged so
that B and D received different outcomes. Random outcomes were assigned to B
and D stimuli in the Control Group. Performance on the TI test will be
examined to determine whether commonly coded stimuli enhance or interfere
with TI performance relative to a non-commonly coded pair of stimuli. |
Tamo |
Tamo Nakamura, Anthony A.
Wright, Jacquelyne J. Rivera (The University of Texas Health Science Center
at Houston), Jeffrey S. Katz, Kent D. Bodily, & Bradley R. Sturz (Auburn
University) |
Training
with Different Initial Set Sizes and Same/Different Concept Learning in
Pigeons |
Four
groups of pigeons were trained initially with 8, 32, 64, and 1024 stimuli.
Following transfer to novel stimuli, set sizes were successively doubled for
each group to 1024 items. Learning rates (initial and overall) did not differ
appreciably across groups. Initial transfer at successive set sizes varied
directly with initial training set size (e.g. the 32-item group transfered
better than the 8-item group at the 32-item set size and the 64-item group
transfered better than the 32-item group at the 64-item set size). All groups
achieved a high level of transfer and concept learning (~80 % correct) by the
time they completed training with the 1024-item set. The results are
discussed in terms of similarities and differences in how these four groups of
pigeons learned the same/different task. |
Treichler |
F. Robert Treichler and Mary Ann
Raghanti (Kent State University) |
Monkeys linking serial lists: A test of episodic
memory |
One
issue in comparative testing of episodic memory is whether serial lists are
retained as multiple associations or as organized cognitive arrays. In
animals and children, such serial memory tests have been employed to assess
the development of transitive (but not logical) inference. Terrace (and others) have reported that
monkeys retain information about the positions of items in serial lists and
organize these as a set of ordinal rankings. These rankings subsequently
determine choice when between-list pairs appear as tests. In our experiments, monkeys seem to
generate integrated ordinal memorial arrays via simple training of pairs that
link between previously learned serial lists.
We will discuss procedures, outcomes, appropriate measures and
limitations of serial list-linking methodology with the goal of encouraging
its use and application in further tests of cognitive performance. |
Vonk |
Jennifer Vonk, James E. Reaux
& Daniel J. Povinelli (University of Louisiana at Lafayette) |
Determinants
of Individual Differences in Cognitive Tasks with Chimpanzees |
Archived
data from seven chimpanzees who have participated in a variety of cognitive
tasks throughout their lifetimes were analyzed to examine individual
differences in performance. Tasks designed to tap into diverse abilities,
such as tool use, concept representation, self recognition, and visual
perspective-taking, were classified according to multiple dimensions, such as
social versus physical problems, choice versus action tasks, prediction
versus explanation paradigms, and competitive versus co-operative tasks. By
ranking the chimpanzees according to multiple measures of task performance,
such as trials to criterion, first trial, transfer and overall performance,
we determined whether individuals displayed consistent strengths and/or
difficulties according to the measures of performance, parameters of the
tasks, and the core knowledge domains presumably tapped into by each task.
Results from these analyses will determine whether performance on various
tasks is best predicted by the overall skill of individuals or whether
chimpanzees as a group perform similarly according to task domains and
parameters. |
Weisman |
Andrea Friedrich, Thomas Zentall
(University of Kentucky), and Ronald Weisman (Queen's University), |
The
Specifics of Absolute Pitch Perception in Pigeons. |
Absolute
pitch perception (AP) refers to the ability to identify, classify, and
memorize pitches without use of an external reference pitch. In previously
published tests of AP, several species of birds and mammals were trained to
sort contiguous tones into frequency ranges, based on correlations between
responding to tones in each frequency range and reinforcement. Here we report
the specifics of our results with pigeons, a species not known for its
auditory expertise. We studied pigeons in three and eight frequency range
discriminations across higher and lower acoustic frequency regions. Our results suggest that pigeons have AP
sorting abilities that are broadly similar to other avian species. However,
pigeons acquire frequency range discriminations more slowly and with greater
variability between individuals than we have observed in other birds. |
Whitlow |
Bill Whitlow (Rutgers University
- Camden) |
Configural
learning in a social reasoning task |
Associative
approaches to causal learning in humans have suggested that people often rely
on configural cues rather than elemental cues in making their judgments. This suggestion was investigated with a
social reasoning task in which associative relations of positive and negative
strength can be mapped onto social relations of active support for or active
antipathy against a target individual.
Social reasoning, about which people may be presumed to be relatively
sophisticated, is compared to reasoning about food allergies, a more
conventional reasoning task. |
Williams |
Douglas A. Williams, Rachel
Cook, Carla Lawson, & Kenneth W. Johns (University of Winnipeg) |
“When” Trumps “Whether” in Pavlovian Appetitive
Conditioning |
Under
a deeply negative contingency, food-restricted rats (rattus norvegicus)
anticipated the arrival time of a single food pellet US presented at a fixed
time after the onset of an extended 2 min white noise CS. Conditioned food-magazine entries by rats
peaked above ITI levels at the scheduled arrival time of the pellet US,
either 30- or 90-s after CS onset, in separate groups, when pellets occurred
four times more frequently on a probabilistic basis during the intertrial
interval (ITI). Some have said that
conditioning involves learning both “whether” the CS signals an increase in
the probability of the US, and if so, “when” the US is scheduled to arrive. These results show that “when” trumps
“whether”, challenging accepted doctrine that a positive contingency between
the CS and US is necessary for CR generation. |
Yeater |
Deirdre Yeater, Stan Kuczaj
(University of Southern Mississippi) & Moby Solangi (Institute for Marine
Mammal Studies) |
Cognitive
and Social Influences on Dolphin Regurgitation |
A
young captive dolphin began to exhibit voluntary regurgitation shortly after
she was moved to a pool containing two other dolphins that regurgitated at
will. In an attempt to better
understand the reasons for such behavior, each dolphin was systematically
observed for a twelve week period.
These results suggested that both cognitive and social factors
influenced voluntary regurgitation.
Some dolphins appeared to regurgitate in order to alleviate
boredom. In such cases, voluntary
regurgitation may be a form of self-enrichment. However, social rank and modeling of more
dominant animals also influenced the frequency of regurgitation, particularly
for the young target dolphin. Thus, both cognitive and social factors affect
voluntary regurgitation by dolphins, and most likely explain why some animals
are prone to regurgitation while others are not. |
Yi |
Linlin Yi (Brown University) |
The
Combination Rule for Simultaneous Timing of Two Independently Reinforced
Fixed-Intervals |
When
animals time more than one interval at the same time, a combination rule is
supposed for the simultaneous timing process. Twelve rats were used in an
instrumental appetitive lever press procedure that involved two independently
reinforced fixed-intervals indicated by a long stimulus (120 s) and a short
stimulus (60 s). Results showed that the short stimulus and the first
reinforcement played dominant roles in the simultaneous timing. Three
possible combination rules are discussed: the short-stimulus-dominance rule,
the first-food-dominance rule, and the weighted sum rule. Analyses suggested
that rats used a weighted sum of the effects of the stimuli, rather than
relying exclusively on the short stimulus or the first reinforcement. |
Young |
Michael E. Young, Joshua S.
Beckmann (Southern Illinois University
at Carbondale) & Edward A. Wasserman (University of Iowa) |
The pigeon's
discrimination of Michotte's launching effect |
In the
present study, pigeons learned to discriminate a Michotte launching animation
from three other launching animations using a go/no-go task. Each pigeon was reinforced for pecking at
one of the interactions but not for pecking at the other three. The four animations constituted two
classes: causal (direct launching) and non-causal (delayed, distal, and
delayed & distal). Two birds
learned to peck at the causal interaction but not at non-causal interactions
whereas two different birds learned to peck at a distal and delayed
interaction but not at the other interactions. Both discriminations were very difficult
for the pigeons and subsequent tests revealed a tendency to solve the
discrimination by attention to subtle stimulus properties. These results suggest that causality in
observed object interactions is not as salient for pigeons as it is for
people. |
Zentall |
Thomas R. Zentall & Rebecca
A. Singer (University of Kentucky) |
Episodic
Memory in Animals Requires the Answer to an Unexpected Question |
For
animals, the answer to an expected question can be framed as a rule or a
semantic memory (e.g., in matching-to-sample; if the sample is red choose the
circle). To distinguish between episodic and semantic memory it is important
that the question asked (e.g., what color was the sample?) be unexpected. To
accomplish this, the test should involve the transfer of training. Maki
(1979) found that pigeons can retrieve memories of food and the absence of
food when unexpectedly requested. And we have found that pigeons can retrieve
examples of their own differential behavior (pecking or not pecking, Zentall,
Cement, Bhatt, & Allen, 2001). We are also investigating whether pigeons
can retrieve a location where they have pecked when unexpectedly asked. We
believe that the ability of pigeon to retrieve hedonic, behavioral, and
spatial events when unexpectedly requested is analogous to human episodic
memory (e.g., the answer to the question, “What did you have for breakfast
this morning?). |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|