Visual Perception
Dr. Robert G. Cook,
Tufts University
Comparative Psychology: A handbook (1998)
Any inventory of the
animal world quickly reveals a bewildering assortment of visual
systems evolved for the purposes of detecting and using
information from reflected light. These range from elementary
photoreceptors that only discriminate light from dark, to the
considerably more complex interactions of eye and brain
responsible for visual perception in birds and mammals. This
ability of nervous systems to construct internal visual
representations of the outside world represents one of the most
important milestones in the evolution of animal behavior and
cognition. "Seeing" has the great advantage of allowing
animals to obtain information concerning the nature and location
of objects in their environment without the need for direct or
close physical contact, as required by more proximal senses like
touch, taste and smell. Because of this, visual information has
become crucial to many animals for locating and identifying food,
suitable habitats, predators, and conspecifics, as well as
functioning to orient animals in their overall surroundings.
The direct physical stimulus for visual perception is
reflected light of differing wavelengths. It is important to keep
in mind that the resulting internal perception of this
stimulation is not only a reflection of its physical properties,
but also the changes induced by its transduction, filtering, and
transformation by the animal's nervous system. Perhaps because of
our strong visual predisposition as a species, the psychological
difficulties inherent in using light to discern the structure of
the external world are not widely appreciated and easily
overlooked. For example, how are three-dimensional perceptual
relations reconstructed from just two-dimensional retinal
information, or how are an object's boundaries properly
determined given all of the luminance contrasts present in any
visual scene? It is the rapid and apparently effortless
resolution of such computational problems that make the brain,
and not the eye, the true organ of visual perception. Given the
brain's very important interpretive role in the construction of
any complex visual impression, it is far more important to be
cognizance of an animal's perceived environment than its
physical environment when trying to understand any
visually-guided behavior. In a closely related point, it is
important to recognize that each species also possesses a
distinctive combination of sensory equipment and perceptual
capacities tuned to the particular demands of its niche. The term
"umvelt" has been employed by ethologists to refer to
these different constellations of perceptual abilities and
priorities across species.
Humans perceive light wavelengths of 400 to 700 nanometers,
for instance, with a peak sensitivity near 555 nanometers. It is
this physical stimulation that eventually results in our
psychological impression of the color spectrum ranging from the
blues to the reds respectively. Our umvelt or filter for
"visible" light often causes us to overlook the fact
that other animals are able to sense wavelengths outside of this
range and into the ultraviolet or infrared regions of the
spectrum. Bees can detect ultraviolet light, for example, which
allows them to see the distinct ultraviolet patterns reflected by
many flowers which act as visual guides to help the bees locate
the flower's nectar. Variations in the umvelt of different
species extend to other visual features besides the perception of
color. The visual acuity of birds of prey, such as the falcon,
easily exceeds our own, allowing these aerial hunters to detect
prey at considerable distances. Pigeons can see patterns of
polarized light in the daytime sky which are invisible to us,
providing yet another possible source of information for the
remarkable homing abilities of these birds. These examples offer
only a glimpse of the kind of visual information available to
various animals, but hopefully demonstrate that our own visual
experience is only a rough guide, at best, to visualizing the
perceptual world of other animals.
With such cautions in mind, comparative psychologists have
made significant experimental progress over the last several
decades towards understanding visual perception and its
underlying mechanisms in animals. In this pursuit, these
scientists have focused on three broad and related sets of
questions. The first set of questions have been directed at
determining the basic visual faculties of animals. The second set
of questions have been more functional in nature, devoted to
asking about the role of different forms of visual information in
an animal's daily survival, and more specifically, the identity
of the effective stimuli controlling these behaviors. The third
set have focused on identifying and analyzing the mechanisms
underlying these perceptions. Explorations of this latter
question have ranged from studies of the anatomy and physiology
of single nerve cells in the visual cortex to investigations of
visual discrimination behavior in individual animals. Because of
the vast wealth of information in the visual sciences, this essay
by necessity must be limited in scope. As such, its goal is to
provide a brief overview of how these different questions have
been advanced by explorations of the visual stimulus control of
behavior.
Identifying the effective stimulus controlling an animal's
behavior is among the oldest and most fundamental of concerns in
comparative psychology. Pursued in both the field and laboratory,
the answer to this question not only advances the functional
analysis of behavior, but indirectly furthers our understanding
of an animal's visual capacities and priorities. The almost
universal tactic in this approach is to appraise how animals
react to variations of the visual input governing a particular
behavior in order to isolate and identify the critical
controlling features. This stimulus-analytic strategy typically
involves a series of tests in which the complex original stimuli
associated with a behavior are decomposed into their simpler
constituent features or configurations to see which are still
capable of maintaining the behavior of interest.
This strategy can be seen in Tinbergen's classic research on
the begging responses of young Hering gulls (Tinbergen, 1951).
Soon after hatching, gull chicks peck at their parent's bill in
order to obtain regurgitated food. Tinbergen evaluated the
effective stimuli controlling this begging response by presenting
the chicks a graded series of cardboard models that mimicked the
parent's head and bill in a variety of ways. The number of
responses elicited from the young gulls by the different models
revealed that the size, action, and position of the bill were all
involved, but perhaps most important was the presence of the
contrasting red spot at the tip of the adult's bill. Importantly,
he also established that not all features of the adult's head
were critical, as neither the color of the model's head or its
bill influenced the strength of the chick's response. The
prey-catching behavior of toads has been similarly subjected to
this same type of analysis by measuring the vigor and number of
responses elicited by systematically varying models of worm-like
stimuli (Ewert, 1987). These studies have revealed that this
animal's visual recognition of prey entails a conjunction of
attributes involving the model's size, shape, and direction of
motion; such that thin elongated stimuli moving along an extended
worm-like axis are considerably more preferred by toads than the
same shape moving perpendicular to this axis.
The selective responsiveness of the toads and young gulls to
particular stimulus features in these functionally-oriented
analyses of behavior provide an important, but limited, picture
of these animals' perceptual aptitudes. Limited to readily
summoned natural behaviors and their associated stimuli, these
types of analyses reveal little about the range and sensitivity
of animals to different forms of visual stimulation. Such
analyses of the psychophysics of perception, which map out in
detail the relations between a subject's psychological reaction
to highly specified sets of physical stimuli, are best conducted
with specially trained animals in the laboratory. This setting
allows for a greater variety of stimuli to be tested, an
increased precision in their description and method of
presentation, and the opportunity to meticulously and repeatedly
measure the animal's response to these stimuli.
Two types of visual discrimination procedures have been
traditionally employed with animals for these psychophysical
examinations of perception. The first involves teaching the
animal a response to a single stimulus, which is then followed by
a series of stimulus-analytic transfer tests examining their
reaction to variations of the original signal. A pigeon might be
trained to respond to a pecking key illuminated with a particular
wavelength, for example, and then presented a variety of other
colors to see how far this pecking response will generalize. The
same tactic can been employed using habituation procedures. After
an animal's response to a particular stimulus has been habituated
through its repeated presentation, the animal's internal
representation of the repeated stimulus is examined by measuring
the amount of dishabituation produced by other stimuli. Again,
the degree of stimulus control maintained by the transfer stimuli
indexes the perceptual and conceptual similarity of these stimuli
to the original. This type of habituation procedure has been
extensively used to study the perceptual world of human infants,
for instance. A second and superior discrimination procedure
involves teaching the animal to differentially behave in the
presence of two or more stimuli in an operant setting; either by
making a response or not, as in a go/no-go procedure, or better
yet requiring the animal to make a choice among two or more
distinct response alternatives associated with these stimuli, as
in a matching-to-sample procedure.
Since the 1950s, these types of learned discriminations have
been used with animals to productively study their spectral
sensitivity, visual acuity, and capacities to detect and
discriminate fundamental visual dimensions such as hue, size,
orientation, and brightness (Berkeley & Stebbins, 1990).
While assembling these standardized measures of basic visual
performance have been an essential step towards understanding any
animal's perceptual world, they leave unanswered many of the most
important and intriguing problems of visual perception and
cognition. Consider for a moment the highly variable and
constantly changing nature of the light falling upon the eye.
Despite the numerous ambiguities and limitations in this changing
input, our brain is still able to reconstruct a stable, unitary,
and three dimensional visual impression of the world. Thus as any
object moves it continues to be perceived and recognized as the
same "thing," despite the continual transforming and
different patterns of light produced by this movement. The exact
computational processes by which the brain solves this
"stimulus equivalence" or "many-to-one"
problem remains a puzzle.
Judging from much of their behavior, however, complex animals
such as birds and mammals act as if they too perceive a stable
perceptual world, where associated collections of form and color
attributes are also consistently interpreted and recognized as
invariant "objects". The mechanisms underlying these
more complicated aspects of visual cognition have become of
increasing interest to animal researchers (Stebbins &
Berkeley, 1990). One important catalyst for this has been the
recent research on natural concept formation by pigeons (e.g.,
Herrnstein, Loveland & Cable, 1976). In such experiments, the
animals learned to discriminate among realistic color photographs
of different classes of objects, such as trees, humans, fish, and
water. Not only were these discriminations between categories
easily and rapidly learned by the pigeons, they also generalized
to novel examples of these categories as well, suggesting a form
of rudimentary conceptual behavior and raising the suspicion that
the birds were perceiving the "objects" depicted in the
slides. These results stimulated a great deal of new research
into the perception and categorization of all types of complex
visual stimuli by animals, and most especially, the pigeon.
One important by-product of this new look at complex stimulus
perception in animals has been its inevitable comparison to human
perception and performance with similar stimuli. Recent studies,
for example, have provided experimental evidence that pigeons,
monkeys, and humans similarly perceive some types of visual
stimuli. Evidence for this shared perception comes from analyses
of the discrimination errors made by each species when
distinguishing among the same stimuli. An example of this can be
found in Sands, Lincoln & Wright's (1982) experiments testing
pictorial perception in rhesus monkeys. In this experiment the
monkey had to judge whether two separate pictures were identical
or not. Testing many pictures of flowers, fruits, monkey and
human faces, the monkeys consistently found images from the same
categories harder to discriminate than those of different
categories, suggesting their perceptual categorization of these
stimuli matched our own grouping of them. Given our shared
primate heritage and similar brain organizations, this similarity
is perhaps not too surprising.
Of greater interest is that pigeons have been shown to produce
comparable results in strategically similar analyses. Blough
(1982) required pigeons to discriminate among different letters
of the alphabet. He found that the pigeons exhibited a pattern of
discrimination errors very much like our own, confusing similar
letters like "O", "Q" and "D" for
instance. Despite the considerable differences in the
organization, size, and natural history of the mammalian and
avian brain, this behavioral similarity suggests a corresponding
internal representation of these particular stimuli. If so, it
raises the interesting question of whether these shared
impressions are the product of analogous or common psychological
algorithms as embodied by different neural architectures, or are
instead generated by different computational processes that
function to the same visual end.
This issue of mechanism has been explored using a variety of
stimuli and behavioral procedures that try to isolate and measure
the different portions of the perceptual process. One way is to
test whether animals experience our visual illusions, since the
"misperceptions" invoked by such stimuli help to
directly reveal the visual system's active contribution to
perception. Pigeons seem to suffer from some of the same
geometric illusions as humans, such as the Ponzo and Mueller-Lyer
illusions (Fujita, Blough, & Blough, 1991; Malott &
Malott, 1970). Besides indicating a common perception, this type
of similarity suggests even further that some of the underlying
processes involved are also the same. In an attempt to
isolate the early visual mechanisms responsible for registering
and discriminating object surfaces and edges, my colleagues and I
have been investigating the phenomenon of perceptual grouping in
pigeons and humans by testing them with different types of
multi-element visual stimuli. The results thus far have
encouraged the view that these visual grouping processes are
similarly organized in both species (Cook, 1992). Other visual
discrimination experiments comparing pigeons and humans, however,
have suggested that important process differences also exist.
Humans differ in how quickly they can find a particular
"target" element in a display depending upon the
elements surrounding it, making it is easier, for example, to
locate a "Q" in a field of many "O"s than
vice versa. These type of asymmetries in the speed of human
visual search help reveal the structure and organization of the
elementary features employed in form perception. Testing pigeons
with similar combinations of Qs and Os, Allan and Blough (1989)
found no evidence of comparable asymmetries in the search
behavior of these animals, raising the possibility that a
different sets of visual features may be emphasized in the avian
and mammalian perception of form.
This essay has tried to weave together something of the
questions, findings, history, methods, and strategies employed in
the comparative investigation of animal visual perception.
Through the various experimental approaches outlined, we have
been gaining an increasingly better understanding and insight of
their internal world. Many of our answers, however, remain
speculative and tentative. As a consequence, they offer an
exciting and open invitation to all students to join in the
scientific search for a better bird's eye view.
Allan, S. E., & Blough, D. S. (1989). Feature-based search
asymmetries in pigeons and humans. Perception &
Psychophysics, 46, 456-464.
Berkeley, M.A., & Stebbins W.C. (1990). Comparative
perception: Complex signals. John Wiley: New York.
Blough, D.S. (1982). Pigeon perception of letters of the
alphabet. Science, 218, 397-398.
Cook, R. G. (1992). Dimensional organization and texture
discrimination in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Animal Behavior Processes, 18, 354-363.
Ewert, J.-P. (1987). Neuroethology of releasing mechanisms:
Prey-catching in toads. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10,
337-405.
Fujita, K., Blough, D.S., & Blough, P.M. (1991). Pigeons
see the Ponzo illusion. Animal Learning & Behavior, 19,
283-293.
Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H., & Cable, C. (1976).
Natural concepts in pigeons. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2,
285-311.
Malott, R. W., & Malott, M. K. (1970). Perception and
stimulus generalization. In W.C. Stebbins (Ed.), Animal
psychophysics: The design and conduct of sensory experiments (pp.
363-400). New York: Plenum.
Sands, S. F., Lincoln, C. E., & Wright, A. A. (1982).
Pictorial similarity judgments and organization of visual memory
in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 4, 369-389.
Stebbins, W. C., & Berkeley, M.A. (1990). Comparative
perception: Basic mechanisms. John Wiley: New York.
Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. Clarendon
Press, Oxford.