Bednekoff, P.A. & Balda, R.P. (1996a). Social
caching and observational spatial memory in pinyon
jays. Behaviour, 133, 807-826.
Bednekoff, P.A., & Balda, R.P. (1996b).
Observational spatial memory in Clark's nutcrackers
and Mexican jays. Animal Behaviour, 52,
833-839.
Benhamou, S., & Bovet, P.
(1992). Distinguishing between elementary
orientation mechanisms by means of path analysis. Animal Behaviour, 43, 371-377.
Benhamou, S., Poucet, B., &
Bovet, P. (1995). A model for place navigation in
mammals. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 173,
163-178.
Biegler, R. (1996). Short and
medium range navigation and its relationship to
cognitive mapping and associative
learning. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of
Edinburgh, Edinburgh, Scotland.
Biegler, R. (2000).
Possible uses of path integration in animal
navigation. Animal Learning & Behavior, 28(3),
257-277.
Biegler, R. (2003). Reading
cognitive and other maps: How to avoid getting
buried in thought. In K. J. Jeffery (Ed.), The
neurobiology of spatial behaviour. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Biegler, R., & Morris, R.G.M.
(1996). Landmark stability: Further studies pointing
to a role in spatial learning. Quarterly Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 49B(4), 307-345.
Blackford-Cook, S. (1969).
Experiments on homing in the limpet Siphonaria
normalis. Animal Behaviour, 17,
679-682.
Brown, M.A., & Sharp, P.E.
(1995). Simulation of spatial learning in the Morris
water maze by a neural network model of the
hippocampus and nucleus accumbens. Hippocampus,
5(3), 171-188.
Brown, S.F., & Mellgren, R.L.
(1994). Distinction between places and paths in
rats' spatial representation. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,
20(1), 20-31.
Bures, J., Buresova, O., & Nerad,
L. (1992). Can rats solve a simple version of the
travelling salesman problem? Behavioural
Brain Research, 52, 133-142.
Buresova, O., Homuta, L.,
Krekule, I., & Bures, J. (1988). Does
non-directional signalization of target distance
contribute to navigation in the Morris water maze? Behavioral and Neural Biology, 49(2), 240-248.
Burgess, N., Recce, M., &
O'Keefe, J. (1994). A model of hippocampal function.
Neural Networks, 7, 1065-1081.
Cartwright, B.A., & Collett, T.S.
(1982). How honey bees use landmarks to guide their
return to food. Nature, 295, 560-564.
Cartwright, B.A, & Collett, T.S.
(1983). Landmark learning in bees: Experiments and
models. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 151, 521-543.
Charnov, E.L. (1976). Optimal
foraging: The marginal value theorem. Theoretical
Population Biology, 9, 129-136.
Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module in the
rat's spatial representation. Cognition, 23,
149–177.
Cheng, K. (1988). Some
psychophysics of the pigeon‘s use of landmarks. Journal
of Comparative Physiology A. 162,
815-826.
Cheng, K. (1990). More
psychophysics of the pigeon‘s use of landmarks. Journal
of Comparative Physiology A. 166,
857-863.
Cheng, K. (2000). How honeybees
find a place: Lessons from a simple mind. Animal Learning & Behavior, 28(1), 1-15.
Cheng, K., Collett, T.S., Pickhard, A., & Wehner, R.
(1987). The use of visual landmarks by honeybees:
Bees weight landmarks according to their distance
from the goal. Journal of Comparative Physiology
A, 161, 469-475.
Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C. R. (1984). Testing the
geometric power of an animal's spatial
representation. In H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Bever, & H.
S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp.
409–423). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cheng, K., & Spetch, M.L. (1998).
Mechanisms of landmarks use in mammals and birds. In
S. D. Healy (Ed.), Spatial representation in
animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chew, G.L., Sutherland, R.J., &
Whishaw, I.Q. (1989). Latent learning does not
produce instantaneous transfer of place navigation:
A rejoinder to Keith and McVety.
Psychobiology, 17, 207-209.
Chittka, L., & Geiger, K.
(1995a). The influences of landmarks on distance
estimation of honey bees. Animal Behaviour, 50,
23-31.
Chittka, L., & Geiger, K.
(1995b). Honeybee long-distance orientation in a
controlled environment. Ethology, 99,
117-126.
Chittka, L., & Kunze, J.
(1995). The significance of landmarks for path
integration in homing honeybee foragers. Naturwissenschaften, 82, 341-343.
Clifford, K.T., Gross, L.,
Johnson, K., Martin, K. J., Shaheen, N., &
Harrington, M.A. (2003). Slime-trail tracking in the
predatory snail, Euglandina rosea. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117(5),
1086-1095.
Collett, M., & Collett, T.S.
(2000). How do insects use path integration for
their navigation? Biological Cybernetics, 83,
245-259.
Collett, M., Collett, T.S.,
Bisch, S., & Wehner, R. (1998). Local and global
vectors in desert ant navigation. Nature,
394, 269-272.
Collett, T.S. (1982). Do
toads plan routes? A study of detour behaviour
in Bufo viridis. Journal of Comparative
Physiology, 146, 261-271.
Collett, T.S. (1995). Making
learning easy: The acquisition of visual information
during the orientation flight of social wasps.
Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 177,
737-747.
Collett, T.S. (1996). Insect
navigation en route to the goal: Multiple strategies
for the use of landmarks. Journal of Experimental
Biology, 199(1), 227-235.
Collett, T.S., & Baron, J.
(1994). Biological compasses and the coordinate
frame of landmark memories in honeybees. Nature,
368, 137-140.
Collett, T.S., Dillmann, E.,
Giger, A., & Wehner, R. (1992). Visual landmarks and
route following in desert ants. Journal of
Comparative Physiology A, 170, 435-442.
Collett, T.S., & Harkness, L.I.K.
(1982). Depth vision in animals. In D. J. Ingle, M.
A. Goodale. & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis
of visual behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cook, A. (1977). Mucus trail
following by the slug Limax grossui Lupu.
Animal Behaviour, 25, 774-781.
Cook, A., Bamford, O.S., Freeman,
J.D.B., & Teidemann, D.J. (1969). A study of the
homing habit of the limpet. Animal Behaviour, 17,
330-339.
Cramer, A.E., & Gallistel, C.R.
(1997). Vervet monkeys as travelling salesmen.
Nature, 387, 464.
Deutsch, J.A. (1960).
The structural basis of behaviour. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Eason. P.K., Cobbs, G.A., &
Trinca, K.G. (1999). The use of landmarks to
define territorial boundaries. Animal Behaviour,
58(1), 85-91.
Etienne, A.S., & Jeffery, K.J.
(2004). Path integration in mammals. Hippocampus,
14(2), 180-192.
Etienne, A.S., Teroni, E., Hurni,
C., & Portenier, V. (1990). The effect of a single
light cue on homing behaviour of the golden hamster. Animal Behaviour, 39, 17-41.
Fitzgerald, T.D. (2003). Role of
trail pheromone in foraging and processionary
behaviour of pine processionary caterpillars.
Thaumetopoea pityocampa. Journal of Chemical Ecology,
29(3), 513-532.
Frisch, K. von (1967). The
dance language and orientation of bees. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Gallistel, C.R. (1990). The
organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T.
Press.
Gehlbach, F. R., Watkins, J. F.,
& Kroll, J. C. (1971). Pheromone
trail-following studies of typhlopid, leptotyphlopid
and colubrid snakes. Behaviour, 40,
282-294.
Gonor, J.J. (1967). Predator-prey
reactions between two marine Prosobranch gastropods. The Veliger, 7, 228-232.
Hartmann, G., & Wehner, R. (1995). The ant's path
integration system: A neural architecture.
Biological Cybernetics, 73, 483–493.
Healy, S.D., & Hurly, T.A.
(1998). Rufous hummingbirds' (Selasphorus rufus)
memory for flowers: Patterns or actual spatial
locations? Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Animal Behavior Processes, 24, 396-404.
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994).
A geometric process for spatial representation in
young children. Nature, 370, 57-59.
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1996).
Modularity and development: The case of spatial
reorientation. Cognition, 61, 195-232.
Jackson, D.E., Holcombe, M., &
Ratnieks, F.L.W. (2004). Trail geometry gives
polarity to ant foraging networks. Nature, 432,
907-909.
Jackson, R.R., & Wilcox, R.S.
(1993). Observations in nature of detouring
behaviour by Portia fimbriata, a web-invading
agressive mimic jumping spider from Queensland. Journal
of Zoology, London, 230, 135-139.
Kamil, A.C., & Jones, J.E.
(1997). Clark's nutcrackers learn geometric
relationships among landmarks. Nature, 390,
276‑279.
Keith, J.R., & McVety, K.M.
(1988). Latent place learning in a novel environment
and the influences of prior training in rats. Psychobiology, 16 (2), 146-151.
Kelly, D., Spetch, M.L., & Heth,
C.D. (1998). Pigeon’s encoding of geometric and
featural properties of a spatial environment.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112,
259-269.
LaManna, J.R., & Eason, P.K.
(2003). Effects of landmarks on territorial
establishment. Animal Behaviour, 65, 471-478.
Lock, A., & Collett, T.S. (1979).
A toad's devious approach to prey: A study of some
complex uses of depth vision. Journal of
Comparative Physiology, 131, 179-189.
Mackintosh, J.H. (1973).
Factors affecting the recognition of territory
boundaries by mice (Mus musculus). Animal Behaviour, 21, 464-470.
McNaughton, B.L., Barnes, C.A., Gerrard, J.L.,
Gothard, K., Jung, M.W., Knierim, J.J., Kudrimoti,
H., Qin, Y., Skaggs, W.E., Suster, M., & Weaver, K.L.
(1996). Deciphering the hippocampal polyglot: The
hippocampus as a path integration system. Journal of
Experimental Biology, 199, 173-185.
Mittelstaedt, H. (2000).
Triple-loop model of path control by head direction
and place cells. Biological Cybernetics, 83,
261-270.
Morris, R.G.M. (1981).
Spatial localisation does not require the presence
of local cues. Learning and Motivation, 12,
239-260.
Müller, M., & Wehner, R. (1988).
Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis
fortis. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
85,
5287-5290.
Muller, R.U., Stead. M., & Pach,
J. (1996). The hippocampus as a cognitive
graph.
Journal of General Physiology, 107, 663-694.
O'Keefe, J. (1976). Place
units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat.
Experimental Neurology, 51, 78-109.
O'Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978).
The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Paine, R. T. (1963). Food
recognition and predation on Opisthobranchs by
Navanax inermis. The Veliger, 6, 1-9.
Pearce, J.M., Good, M.A., Jones,
P.M., & McGregor, A. (2004). Transfer of spatial
behaviour between different environments:
Implications for theories of spatial learning and
for the role of the hippocampus in spatial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior
Processes, 30(2), 135-147.
Reid, A.K., & Staddon, J.E.R. (1998). A dynamic
route finder for the congitive map. Psychological
Review, 105, 585-601.
Rescorla, R.A. (1991).
Associative relations in instrumental learning: The
eighteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B, 1-23.
Rotenberg, A., & Muller, R.U.
(1997). Variable
place-cell coupling to a continuously viewed
stimulus: Evidence that the hippocampus acts as a
perceptual system. Philosophical Transactions of
the Royal Society of London B, 352,
1505-1513.
Samsonovich, A., & McNaughton, B.
L. (1997). Path integration and cognitive mapping in
a continuous attractor neuronal network model.
Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 5900-5920.
Schölkopf, B., & Mallot, H.A.
(1994). View-based cognitive mapping and path
planning. Adaptive Behaviour, 3(3),
311-348.
Séguinot, V., Maurer, R., &
Etienne, A.S. (1993). Dead reckoning in a small
mammal: The evaluation of distance. Journal of
Comparative Physiology A, 173, 103-113.
Skaggs, W.E., Knierim, J.J., Kudrimoti, H.S., &
McNaughton, B.L. (1995). A model of the neural basis
of the rat’s sense of direction. In G. Tesauro, D.
Touretzky, & T. Leen (Eds.) Advances in neural
information processing systems (Vol.
7). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Spetch, M.L., Cheng, K., &
Macdonald, S.E. (1996). Learning the configuration
of a landmark array I: Touch-screen studies with
pigeons and humans. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 110(1), 55-68.
Spetch, M.L., Cheng, K.,
Macdonald, S.E., Linkenhoker, B. E., Kelly, D. M., &
Doerkson, S. R. (1997). Learning the configuration
of a landmark array: II. Generality across search
tasks. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
111(1), 14-24.
Steinhage, A., & Schöner, G.
(1996). Self-calibration based on invariant view
recognition: Dynamic approach to navigation.
Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 20,
133-156.
Sutherland, R.J., Chew, G. L.,
Baker, J.C., & Linggard, R.C. (1987). Some
limitations on the use of distal cues in place
navigation by rats. Psychobiology,
15 (1), 48-57.
Tarsitano, M.S., & Jackson, R.R.
(1997). Araneophagic jumping spiders discriminate
between detours that do and do not lead to prey. Animal
Behaviour, 53(2), 257-266.
Touretzky, D.S., & Redish, A.D. (1996). Theory of
rodent navigation based on interacting
representations of space. Hippocampus, 6,
247-270.
Wan, R.Q., Pang, K., & Olton, D.S. (1994).
Hippocampal and amygdaloid involvement in nonspatial
and spatial working memory in rats: Effects of delay
and interference. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108,
866-882.
Webb, J.K., & Shine, R. (1992).
To find an ant: Trail-following in Australian
blindsnakes (Typhlopidae). Animal Behaviour,
43, 941-948.
Whishaw, I.Q. (1991). Latent
leaning in a swimming pool place task by rats:
Evidence for the use of associative and not
cognitive mapping processes. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 43B, 83-103.