Cheng & Newcombe References

Benhamou, S., & Poucet, B. (1998). Landmark use by navigating rats (Rattus norvegicus): Contrasting geometric and featural information. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 317-322.

Burgess, N., Jackson, A., Hartley, T., & O’Keefe, J. (2000). Predictions derived from modeling the hippocampal role in navigation. Biological Cybernetics, 83, 301-312.

Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. (2003). Neural representations in human spatial memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 517-519.

Chamizo, V.D. (2003). Acquisition of knowledge about spatial location: Assessing the generality of the mechanism of learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56B, 102-113.

Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition, 23, 149-178.

Cheng, K. (2005a). Reflections on geometry and navigation. Connection Science, 17, 5-21.

Cheng, K. (2005b). Goldfish matching geometric and featural cues: A re-interpretation of some of the data of Vargas et al. (2004). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 455-457.

Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C.R. (1984). Testing the geometric power of a spatial representation. In H.L. Roitblat, H.S. Terrace, & T.G. Bever (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 409-423). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C.R. (2005). Shape parameters explain data from spatial transformations: Comment on Pearce et al. (2004) and Tommasi, & Polli (2004). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 254-259.

Cheng, K., & Newcombe, N.S. (2005). Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 1-23.

Dudchenko, P. (2003). The head direction system and navigation. In K.J. Jeffery (Ed.), The neurobiology of spatial behaviour (pp. 173-186). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Egerton, S., Callaghan, V., & Chernett, P. (2000). A biologically inspired mapping model for autonomous mobile robots. In M. Mohammadin (Ed.), New frontiers in computational intelligence and its applications (pp. 20-29). Amsterdam: IOS Press.

Ekstrom, A.D., Kahana, M.J., Caplan, J.B., Fields, T.A., Isham, E.A., Newman, E.L., et al. (2003). Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature, 425, 184-187.

Epstein, R., DeYoe, E.A., Press, D.Z., Rosen, A.C., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). Neuropsychological evidence for a topographical learning mechanism in parahippocampal cortex. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 481-508.

Epstein, R., Graham, K.S., & Downing, P.E. (2003). Viewpoint-specific scene representations in human parahippocampal cortex. Neuron, 37, 865-876.

Epstein, R., Harris, A., Stanley, D., & Kanwisher, N. (1999). The parahippocampal place area: Recognition, navigation, or encoding? Neuron, 23, 115-125.

Epstein, R., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature, 392, 598-601.

Fodor, J.A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fritsch, D.S., Pizer, S.M., Morse, B.S., Eberly, D.H., & Liu, A. (1994). The multiscale medial axis and its applications in image registration.  Pattern Recognition Letters, 15, 445-452.

Gallistel, C.R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.

Golob, E.J., Stackman, R.W., Wong, A.C., & Taube, J.S. (2001). On the behavioral significance of head direction cells: Neural and behavioral dynamics during spatial memory tasks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 285-304.

Gouteux, S., Thinus-Blanc, C., & Vauclair, J. (2001). Rhesus monkeys use geometric and non-geometric information during a reorientation task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 505-519.

Graham, M., Good, M. A., McGregor, A., & Pearce, J. M. (2006). Spatial learning based on the shape of the environment is influenced by properties of the objects forming the shape. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 44-59.

Gray, E.R., Bloomfield, L.L., Ferrey, A., Spetch, M.L., & Sturdy, C.B. (2005). Spatial encoding in mountain chickadees: Features overshadow geometry. Biology Letters, 1, 314-317.

Hartley, T., Burgess, N., Lever, C., Cacucci, F., & O’Keefe, J. (2000). Modelling place fields in terms of the cortical inputs to the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 10, 369-379.

Hartley, T., Maguire, E.A., Spiers, H.J., & Burgess, N. (2003). The well-worn route and the path less travelled: Distinct neural bases of route following and way finding in humans. Neuron, 37, 877-888.

Hartley, T., Trinkler, I., & Burgess, N. (2004). Geometric determinants of human spatial memory. Cognition, 94, 39-75.

Hayward, A., McGregor, A., Good, M.A., & Pearce, J.M. (2003). Absence of overshadowing and blocking between landmarks and geometric cues provided by the shape of a test arena. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56B, 114-126.

Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994). A geometric process for spatial representation in young children. Nature, 370, 57-59.

Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1996). Modularity and development: The case of spatial reorientation. Cognition, 61, 195-232.

Hermer-Vazquez, L., Spelke, E., & Katsnelson, A. (1999). Source of flexibility in human cognition: Dual task studies of space and language. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 3-36.

Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M. (2003). How toddlers represent enclosed spaces. Cognitive Science, 27, 749-766.

Jacobs, L.F., & Schenk, F. (2003). Unpacking the cognitive map: The parallel map theory of hippocampal function. Psychological Review, 110, 285-315.

Jeffery, K.J. (Ed.) (2003). The neurobiology of spatial behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Kelly, D.M., & Spetch, M.L. (2001). Pigeons encode relative geometry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 417-422.

Kelly, D.M., & Spetch, M.L. (2004a). Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment I: Do adults encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 82-94.

Kelly, D.M., & Spetch, M.L. (2004b). Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: II. Do pigeons (Columba livia) encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room?  Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 384-395.

Kelly, D.M., Spetch, M.L., & Heth, C.D. (1998). Pigeon’s encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 259-269.

Learmonth, A.E., Nadel, L., & Newcombe, N.S. (2002). Children’s use of landmarks: Implications for modularity theory. Psychological Science, 13, 337-341.

Learmonth, A.E., Newcombe, N.S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2001). Toddlers’ use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 225-244.

Lever, C., Wills, T., Caccucci, F., Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. (2002). Long term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature, 416, 90-94.

Leyton, M. (1992). Symmetry, causality, mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Maguire, E.A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J.G., Frackowiak, R.S.J., Frith, C.D., & O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing where and getting there: A human navigation network. Science, 280, 921-924.

Maguire, E.A., Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. (1999). Human spatial navigation: Cognitive maps, sexual dimorphism, and neural substrates. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 171-177.

Maguire, E.A., Frith, C.D., Burgess, N., Donnett, J.G., & O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing where things are: Parahippocampal involvement in encoding object locations in virtual large-scale space. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 61-76.

Margules, J., & Gallistel, C.R. (1988). Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape. Animal Learning & Behavior, 16, 404-410.

Marr, D. (1982). Vision. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

McGregor, A., Jones, P.M., Good, M.A., & Pearce, J.M. (2006). Further evidence that rats rely on local rather than global spatial information to locate a hidden goal: Reply to Cheng and Gallistel (2005). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 314-321.

Newcombe, N.S. (2002). The nativist-empiricist controversy in the context of recent research on spatial and quantitative development. Psychological Science, 13, 395-401.

Nolfi, S. (2002). Power and limits of reactive agents. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 42, 119-145.

O'Keefe, J., & Burgess, N. (1996). Geometric determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons.  Nature, 381, 425-428.

O’Keefe, J., Burgess, N., Donnett, J.G., Jeffery, K.J., & Maguire, E.A. (1999). Place cells, navigational accuracy, and the human hippocampus. In G.W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space, and action: Studies in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 153-164). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

O‘Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Pearce, J.M., Good, M.A., Jones, P.M., & McGregor, A. (2004). Transfer of spatial behaviour between different environments: Implications for theories of spatial learning and for the roøe of the hippocampus in spatial learning.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 30(2), 135-147.

Pearce, J.M., Graham, M., Good, M.A., Jones, P.M., & McGregor, A. (2006). Potentiation, overshadowing, and blocking of spatial learning based on the shape of the environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 201-214.

Pearce, J.M., Ward-Robinson, J., Good, M., Fussell, C., & Aydin, A. (2001). Influence of a beacon on spatial learning based on the shape of the test environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 329-344.

Pizer, S.M., Fritsch, D.S., Yushkevich, P.A., Johnson, V.E., & Chaney, E.L. (1999). Segmentation, registration, and measurement of shape variation via image object shape.  IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 18, 851-865.

Poucet, B., Lenck-Santini, P.P., & Save, E. (2003). Drawing parallels between the behavioural and neural properties of navigation. In K.J. Jeffery (Ed.), The neurobiology of spatial behaviour (pp. 187-198). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Sovrano, V.A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2002). Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: Encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish. Cognition, 85, B51-B59.

Sovrano, V.A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2003). Modularity as a fish (Xenotoca eiseni) views it: Conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial reorientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 29, 199-210.

Tommasi, L., Gagliardo, A., Andrew, R.J., & Vallortigara, G. (2003). Separate processing mechanisms for encoding geometric and landmark information in the avian brain. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1695-1702.

Tommasi, L., & Polli, C. (2004). Representation of two geometric features of the environment in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Animal Cognition, 7, 53-59.

Tommasi, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2000). Searching for the center: Spatial cognition in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 477-486.

Tommasi, L., Vallortigara, G., & Zanforlin, M. (1997). Young chicks learn to localize the center of a spatial environment. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 180, 567-572.

Vallortigara, G., Pagni, P., & Sovrano, V.A. (2004). Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: Evidence from a lopsided animal brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 390-400.

Vallortigara, G., Zanforlin, M., & Pasti, G. (1990). Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: A test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 248-254.

Vargas, J.P., López, J.C., Salas, C., & Thinus-Blanc, C. (2004). Encoding of geometric and featural information by goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 206-216.

Vargas, J.P., Petruso, E.J., & Bingman, V.P. (2004). Hippocampal formation and geometric navigation in pigeons. European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1937-1944.

Vargas, J.P., & López, J.C. (2005). Different ways of encoding geometric information by goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 458-460. 

Wall, P.L., Botly, L.C.P., Black, C.K., & Shettleworth, S.J. (2004). The geometric module in the rat: Independence of shape and feature learning in a food finding task. Learning & Behavior, 32, 289-298.

Wang, R.F., & Spelke, E.S. (2002). Human spatial representation: Insights from animals. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 376-382.

Wang, R.F., & Spelke, E.S. (2003). Comparative approaches to human navigation. In K.J. Jeffery (Ed.), The neurobiology of spatial behaviour (pp. 119-143). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Webb, B. (2000). What does robotics offer animal behaviour? Animal Behaviour, 60, 545-558.

Yeap, W.K., & Jefferies, M.E. (1999). Computing representations of the local environment. Artificial Intelligence, 107, 265-301.