Benhamou, S., & Poucet, B. (1998). Landmark use by navigating
rats (Rattus norvegicus): Contrasting geometric and
featural information. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
112, 317-322.
Burgess, N., Jackson, A., Hartley, T., & O’Keefe, J. (2000).
Predictions derived from modeling the hippocampal role in
navigation. Biological Cybernetics, 83, 301-312.
Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. (2003). Neural representations in
human spatial memory. Trends in Cognitive Science,
7, 517-519.
Chamizo, V.D. (2003). Acquisition of
knowledge about spatial location: Assessing the generality of
the mechanism of learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 56B, 102-113.
Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module
in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition, 23,
149-178.
Cheng, K. (2005a). Reflections on geometry and navigation.
Connection Science,
17, 5-21.
Cheng, K. (2005b). Goldfish matching
geometric and featural cues: A re-interpretation of some of the
data of Vargas et al. (2004). Journal of Comparative
Psychology,
119, 455-457.
Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C.R. (1984). Testing the geometric power
of a spatial representation. In H.L. Roitblat, H.S. Terrace, &
T.G. Bever (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 409-423).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C.R. (2005). Shape parameters explain
data from spatial transformations: Comment on Pearce et al.
(2004) and Tommasi, & Polli (2004). Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 254-259.
Cheng, K., & Newcombe, N.S. (2005). Is there a geometric module
for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 1-23.
Dudchenko, P. (2003). The head direction
system and navigation. In K.J. Jeffery (Ed.), The
neurobiology of spatial behaviour (pp. 173-186). Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Egerton, S., Callaghan, V., & Chernett, P.
(2000). A biologically inspired mapping model for autonomous
mobile robots. In M. Mohammadin (Ed.), New frontiers in
computational intelligence and its applications (pp. 20-29).
Amsterdam: IOS Press.
Ekstrom, A.D., Kahana, M.J., Caplan, J.B.,
Fields, T.A., Isham, E.A., Newman, E.L., et al. (2003). Cellular
networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature,
425, 184-187.
Epstein, R., DeYoe, E.A., Press, D.Z., Rosen,
A.C., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). Neuropsychological evidence for a
topographical learning mechanism in parahippocampal cortex.
Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 481-508.
Epstein, R., Graham, K.S., & Downing, P.E.
(2003). Viewpoint-specific scene representations in human
parahippocampal cortex. Neuron, 37, 865-876.
Epstein, R., Harris, A., Stanley, D., &
Kanwisher, N. (1999). The parahippocampal place area:
Recognition, navigation, or encoding? Neuron, 23,
115-125.
Epstein, R., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). A
cortical representation of the local visual environment.
Nature, 392, 598-601.
Fodor, J.A. (1983). The modularity of mind.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fritsch, D.S., Pizer, S.M., Morse, B.S.,
Eberly, D.H., & Liu, A. (1994). The multiscale medial axis and
its applications in image registration. Pattern Recognition
Letters, 15, 445-452.
Gallistel, C.R. (1990). The organization
of learning. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
Golob, E.J., Stackman, R.W., Wong, A.C., &
Taube, J.S. (2001). On the behavioral significance of head
direction cells: Neural and behavioral dynamics during spatial
memory tasks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115,
285-304.
Gouteux, S., Thinus-Blanc, C., & Vauclair, J.
(2001). Rhesus monkeys use geometric and non-geometric
information during a reorientation task. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 505-519.
Graham, M., Good, M. A.,
McGregor, A., & Pearce, J. M. (2006). Spatial learning based on
the shape of the environment is influenced by properties of the
objects forming the shape. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 44-59.
Gray, E.R., Bloomfield,
L.L., Ferrey, A., Spetch, M.L., & Sturdy, C.B. (2005). Spatial
encoding in mountain chickadees: Features overshadow geometry.
Biology Letters, 1, 314-317.
Hartley, T., Burgess, N., Lever, C., Cacucci,
F., & O’Keefe, J. (2000). Modelling place fields in terms of the
cortical inputs to the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 10,
369-379.
Hartley, T., Maguire, E.A., Spiers, H.J., &
Burgess, N. (2003). The well-worn route and the path less
travelled: Distinct neural bases of route following and way
finding in humans. Neuron, 37, 877-888.
Hartley, T., Trinkler, I., & Burgess, N.
(2004). Geometric determinants of human spatial memory.
Cognition, 94, 39-75.
Hayward, A., McGregor, A., Good, M.A., &
Pearce, J.M. (2003). Absence of overshadowing and blocking
between landmarks and geometric cues provided by the shape of a
test arena. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
56B, 114-126.
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994). A geometric
process for spatial representation in young children. Nature,
370, 57-59.
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1996). Modularity
and development: The case of spatial reorientation. Cognition,
61, 195-232.
Hermer-Vazquez, L., Spelke, E., & Katsnelson,
A. (1999). Source of flexibility in human cognition: Dual task
studies of space and language. Cognitive Psychology,
39, 3-36.
Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M. (2003). How
toddlers represent enclosed spaces. Cognitive Science,
27, 749-766.
Jacobs, L.F., & Schenk, F. (2003). Unpacking
the cognitive map: The parallel map theory of hippocampal
function. Psychological Review, 110, 285-315.
Jeffery, K.J. (Ed.) (2003). The
neurobiology of spatial behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Kelly, D.M., & Spetch, M.L. (2001). Pigeons
encode relative geometry. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 417-422.
Kelly, D.M., & Spetch, M.L. (2004a).
Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment I: Do adults
encode the featural and geometric properties of a
two-dimensional schematic of a room? Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 118, 82-94.
Kelly, D.M., & Spetch, M.L. (2004b).
Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: II. Do pigeons (Columba
livia) encode the featural and geometric properties of a
two-dimensional schematic of a room? Journal of
Comparative Psychology, 118, 384-395.
Kelly, D.M., Spetch, M.L., & Heth, C.D. (1998).
Pigeon’s encoding of geometric and featural properties of a
spatial environment. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
112, 259-269.
Learmonth, A.E., Nadel, L., & Newcombe, N.S.
(2002). Children’s use of landmarks: Implications for modularity
theory. Psychological Science, 13, 337-341.
Learmonth, A.E., Newcombe, N.S., &
Huttenlocher, J. (2001). Toddlers’ use of metric information and
landmarks to reorient. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 80, 225-244.
Lever, C., Wills, T., Caccucci, F., Burgess,
N., & O’Keefe, J. (2002). Long term plasticity in hippocampal
place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature,
416, 90-94.
Leyton, M. (1992). Symmetry, causality,
mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Maguire, E.A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J.G.,
Frackowiak, R.S.J., Frith, C.D., & O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing
where and getting there: A human navigation network. Science,
280, 921-924.
Maguire, E.A., Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J.
(1999). Human spatial navigation: Cognitive maps, sexual
dimorphism, and neural substrates. Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, 9, 171-177.
Maguire, E.A., Frith, C.D., Burgess, N.,
Donnett, J.G., & O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing where things are:
Parahippocampal involvement in encoding object locations in
virtual large-scale space. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
10, 61-76.
Margules, J., & Gallistel, C.R. (1988).
Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape.
Animal Learning & Behavior, 16, 404-410.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
McGregor, A., Jones, P.M.,
Good, M.A., & Pearce, J.M. (2006). Further evidence that rats
rely on local rather than global spatial information to locate a
hidden goal: Reply to Cheng and Gallistel (2005). Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32,
314-321.
Newcombe, N.S. (2002). The
nativist-empiricist controversy in the context of recent
research on spatial and quantitative development.
Psychological Science, 13, 395-401.
Nolfi, S. (2002). Power and limits of
reactive agents. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 42,
119-145.
O'Keefe, J., & Burgess, N. (1996). Geometric
determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons.
Nature, 381, 425-428.
O’Keefe, J., Burgess, N., Donnett, J.G.,
Jeffery, K.J., & Maguire, E.A. (1999). Place cells, navigational
accuracy, and the human hippocampus. In G.W. Humphreys, J.
Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space, and action:
Studies in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 153-164). Oxford,
U.K.: Oxford University Press.
O‘Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The
hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Pearce, J.M., Good, M.A., Jones, P.M., &
McGregor, A. (2004). Transfer of spatial behaviour between
different environments: Implications for theories of spatial
learning and for the roøe of the hippocampus in spatial
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 30(2), 135-147.
Pearce, J.M., Graham, M.,
Good, M.A., Jones, P.M., & McGregor, A. (2006). Potentiation,
overshadowing, and blocking of spatial learning based on the
shape of the environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 201-214.
Pearce, J.M., Ward-Robinson, J., Good, M., Fussell, C., & Aydin,
A. (2001). Influence of a beacon on spatial learning based on
the shape of the test environment. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 329-344.
Pizer, S.M., Fritsch, D.S., Yushkevich, P.A.,
Johnson, V.E., & Chaney, E.L. (1999). Segmentation,
registration, and measurement of shape variation via image
object shape. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 18,
851-865.
Poucet, B., Lenck-Santini, P.P., & Save, E.
(2003). Drawing parallels between the behavioural and neural
properties of navigation. In K.J. Jeffery (Ed.), The
neurobiology of spatial behaviour (pp. 187-198). Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Sovrano, V.A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2002).
Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: Encoding
of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial
environment by fish. Cognition, 85, B51-B59.
Sovrano, V.A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2003).
Modularity as a fish (Xenotoca eiseni) views it:
Conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial
reorientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 29, 199-210.
Tommasi, L., Gagliardo, A., Andrew, R.J., &
Vallortigara, G. (2003). Separate processing mechanisms for
encoding geometric and landmark information in the avian brain.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1695-1702.
Tommasi, L., & Polli, C. (2004).
Representation of two geometric features of the environment in
the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Animal Cognition,
7, 53-59.
Tommasi, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2000).
Searching for the center: Spatial cognition in the domestic
chick (Gallus gallus). Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26,
477-486.
Tommasi, L., Vallortigara, G., & Zanforlin,
M. (1997). Young chicks learn to localize the center of a
spatial environment. Journal of Comparative Physiology A,
180, 567-572.
Vallortigara, G., Pagni, P., & Sovrano, V.A.
(2004). Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial
reorientation: Evidence from a lopsided animal brain. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 390-400.
Vallortigara, G., Zanforlin, M., & Pasti, G.
(1990). Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: A
test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Journal of
Comparative Psychology, 104, 248-254.
Vargas, J.P., López, J.C., Salas, C., &
Thinus-Blanc, C. (2004). Encoding of geometric and featural
information by goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal
of Comparative Psychology, 118, 206-216.
Vargas, J.P., Petruso, E.J., & Bingman, V.P.
(2004). Hippocampal formation and geometric navigation in
pigeons. European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1937-1944.
Vargas, J.P., & López,
J.C. (2005). Different ways of encoding geometric information by
goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 119, 458-460.
Wall, P.L., Botly, L.C.P., Black, C.K., &
Shettleworth, S.J. (2004). The geometric module in the rat:
Independence of shape and feature learning in a food finding
task. Learning & Behavior, 32, 289-298.
Wang, R.F., & Spelke, E.S. (2002). Human
spatial representation: Insights from animals. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 6, 376-382.
Wang, R.F., & Spelke, E.S. (2003).
Comparative approaches to human navigation. In K.J. Jeffery
(Ed.), The neurobiology of spatial behaviour (pp.
119-143). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Webb, B. (2000). What does robotics offer
animal behaviour? Animal Behaviour, 60, 545-558.
Yeap, W.K., & Jefferies, M.E. (1999).
Computing representations of the local environment.
Artificial Intelligence, 107, 265-301.