Albert, W.S., & Golledge, R.G. (1998). The use of spatial cognitive
abilities in geographic information systems: The map overlay
operation. Transactions in GIS, 3(1), 7-21.
Allen, G.L. (2000). Men and women, maps and minds: Cognitive bases
of sex-related differences in reading and interpreting maps. In S.
O'Nuallain (Ed.), Spatial cognition: Foundations and
applications (pp. 3-18). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Avraamides, M.N., Klatzky, R.L., Loomis, J.M., & Golledge, R.G.
(2004). Use of cognitive versus perceptual heading during imagined
locomotion depends on the response mode. Psychological Science,
15(6), 403-408.
Barsalou, LW. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral &
Brain Sciences, 22, 577-660.
Bennett, A.T.D. (1996). Do animals have cognitive maps? The
Journal of Experimental Biology, 1999, 219-224.
Bilge, A.R., & Taylor, H.A. (2006). Learning nested environment from
maps: Is spatial updating simultaneous? Unpublished data. Tufts
University.
Bower, G.H., & Morrow, D.G. (1990). Mental models in narrative
comprehension. Science, 247(4938), 44-48.
Bower, G.H., & Rinck, M. (2001). Selecting one among many referents
in spatial situation models. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 81-98.
van
den Broek, P., Rapp, D.N., & Kendeou, P. (2005). Integrating
memory-based and constructionist processes in accounts of reading
comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39, 299-316.
van
den Broek, P., Risden, K., Fletcher, C.R., & Thurlow, R. (1996). A
"landscape" view of reading: Fluctuating patterns of activation and
the construction of a stable memory representation. In B. K. Britton
& A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Models of understanding text (pp.
165-187). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Brunye, T.T., Taylor, H.A., & Rapp, D.N. (2005). Putting things
together: Memory mechanisms underlying the benefits of multimedia
learning. Paper presented at the 46th Annual Meeting of the
Psychonomic Society, Toronto, Ontario.
Carlson-Radvansky, L.A., & Radvansky, G.A. (1996). The influence of
functional relations on spatial term selection. Psychological
Science, 7(1), 56-60.
Cohen, P., Johnston, M., McGee, D., Oviatt, S.,
Clow, J., & Smith, I. (1998). The efficiency of multimodal
interaction: A case study. Paper presented at the International
Conference on Spoken Language Processes, Australia.
deVega, M. (1995). Backward updating of mental
models during continuous reading of narratives. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21,
373-385.
Diwadkar, V.A., & McNamara, T.P. (1997).
Viewpoint dependence in scene recognition. Psychological Science,
8(4), 302-307.
Easton, R.D., & Sholl, M.J. (1995). Object-array
structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 21(2), 483-500.
Ehrlich, K., & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1982).
Spatial descriptions and referential continuity. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(3), 296-306.
Farrell, M.J., & Robertson, I.H. (1998). Mental
rotation and automatic updating of body-centered spatial relations.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 24(1), 227-233.
Foo, P., Wareen, W.H., Duchon, A., & Tarr, M.J.
(2005). Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map-
versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(2),
195-215.
Gallistel, C.R. (1990). The organization of
learning. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
Gattis, M. (2001). Spatial schemas and
abstract thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gauvain, M. (2001). Cultural tools, social
interaction and the development of thinking. Human Development,
44, 126-143.
Gentner, D., & Stevens, A.L. (1983). Mental
models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gernsbacher, M.A. (1990). Language
comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Gilmartin, P. (1986). Maps, mental imagery, and
gender in the recall of geographic information. The American
Cartographer, 13, 335-344.
Givon, T. (1992). The grammar of referential
coherence as mental processing instructions. Linguistics. Special
topic: From grammar to discourse, 30(1), 5-55.
Glasgow, J., Narayanan, N.H., & Chandrasekaran,
B. (Eds.). (1995). Diagrammatic reasoning: Cognitive and
computational perspectives. Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press.
Golledge, R.G., & Spector, N.A. (1978).
Comprehending the urban environment: Theory and practice.
Geographical Analysis, 14, 305-325.
Gouteux, S., & Spelke, E.S. (2001). Children's
use of geometry and landmarks to reorient in an open space.
Cognition, 81, 119-148.
Hakala, C. (1999). Accessibility of spatial
information in a situation model. Discourse Processes, 27,
261-280.
Haviland, S.E., & Clark, H.H. (1974). What's
new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13(5), 512-521.
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994). A geometric
process for spatial representation in young children. Nature,
370, 57-59.
Jackendoff, R., & Landau, B. (1991). Spatial
language and spatial cognition. In D. J. Napoli & J. A. Kegl (Eds.),
Bridges between psychology and linguistics: A swarthmore
festschrift for lila gleitman (pp. 145-169). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983). Mental models:
Toward a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The
simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky
(Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases
(pp. 201-208). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kendeou, P., Rapp, D.N., & van den Broek, P.
(2004). The influence of readers' prior knowledge on text
comprehension and learning from text. In R. Nata (Ed.), Progress
in education (pp. 189-209). New York: Nova Science Publishers,
Inc.
Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2005). The
effects of reader's misconceptions on comprehension of scientific
text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 235-245.
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T.A. (1978). Toward a
model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review,
85(5), 363-394.
Kuipers, B. (2000). The spatial semantic
hierarchy. Artificial Intelligence, 119, 191-233.
Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). "What" and
"where" in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral &
Brain Sciences, 16(2), 217-264.
Leiser, D., & Zilbershatz, A. (1989). The traveller: A computational
model of spatial network learning. Environment and Behavior, 21,
435-463.
Levelt, W.J.M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Linde,
C., & Labov, W. (1975). Spatial networks as a site for the study of
language and thought. Language, 51(4), 924-939.
MacEachren, A.M. (1994). Some truth with maps: A primer on
symbolization and design. Washington, D.C.: Assocation of
American Geographers.
MacEachren, A.M. (1995). How maps work: Representation,
visualization, and design. New York: Guilford Press.
May, M. (2004). Imaginal perspective switches in remembered
environments: Transformation versus interference accounts.
Cognitive Psychology, 48(2), 163-206.
Mayer, R.E. (2001). Multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Mayer, R.E. (2003). The promise of multimedia learning: Using the
same instruction design methods across different media. Learning
and Instruction, 13(2), 125-139.
Mayer, R.E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on
multimedia learning: When presenting more material results in less
understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(1),
187-198.
Mayer, R.E., & Sims, V.K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a
thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multimedia
learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 389-401.
McNamara, T.P., Hardy, J.K., & Hirtle, S.C. (1989). Subjective
hierarchies in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 211-227.
McNamara, T.P., Rump, B., & Werner, S. (2003). Egocentric and
geocentric frames of reference in memory of large-scale space.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 589-595.
McNamara, T. P., & Shelton, A. L. (2003). Cognitive maps and the
hippocampus. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(8), 333-335.
Miller, G.A., & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1976). Language and
perception. Oxford, England: Harvard University Press.
Montello, D.R., Lovelace, K., Golledge, R.G., & Self, C. (1999).
Sex-related differences and similarities in geographic and
environmental spatial abilities. Annals of the Association of
American Geographers, 89, 515-534.
Morrow, D.G. (1994). Spatial models created from text. In H. van
Oostendorp & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Naturalistic text comprehension
(pp. 57-78). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Morrow, D.G., Bower, G.H., & Greenspan, S.L. (1989). Updating
situation models during narrative comprehension. Journal of
Memory and Language, 28, 292-312.
Morrow, D.G., Greenspan, S.L., & Bower, G.H. (1987). Accessibility
and situation model narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory
and Language, 26, 165-187.
Mou,
W., & McNamara, T.P. (2002). Intrinsic frames of reference in
spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 162-170.
Newcombe, N.S., & Sluzenski, J. (2004). Knowing where things are in
the second year of life: Implications for hippocampal development.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. Special Issue on
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1443-1451.
O‘Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The
hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Plumert, J. M., Ewert, K., & Spear, S.J. (1995). The early
development of children's communication about nested spatial
relations. Child Development, 66, 959-969.
Presson, C. C., & Montello, D. R. (1994). Updating after rotational
and translational body movement: Coordinate structure of
perspective space. Perception, 23(12), 1447-1455.
Rapp,
D.N. (2005). Mental models: Theoretical issues for visualizations
in science education. In J. K. Gilbert (Ed.), Visualization in
science education (pp. 43-60). The Netherlands: Springer.
Rapp,
D.N., Klug, J.L., & Taylor, H.A. (in press). Expectation-driven
effects on spatial mental model use during reading. Memory &
Cognition.
Rapp,
D.N., & Taylor, H.A. (2004). Interactive dimensions in the
construction of mental representastions for text. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30,
988-1001.
Rapp,
D.N., Taylor, H.A., & Crane, G. (2003). The impact of digital
libraries on cognitive processes: Psychological issues of
hypermedia. Computers in Human Behavior, 19, 609-628.
Rapp,
D.N., & van den Broek, P. (2005). Dynamic text comprehension: An
integrative view of reading. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 14, 276-279.
Rich,
S.S., & Taylor, H.A. (2000). Not all narrative shifts function
equally. Memory & Rieser, J.J. (1989). Access to knowledge of
spatial structure at novel points of observation. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6),
1157-1165.
Rieser, J.J., Garing, A.E., & Young, M.F. (1994). Imagery, action,
and young children's spatial orientation: It's not being there that
counts, it's what one has in mind. Child Development, 65(5),
1262-1278.
Rinck,
M., & Bower, G.H. (1995). Anaphora resolution and the focus of
attention in situation models. Journal of Memory and Language, 34,
110-131.
Rinck,
M., Bower, G.H., & Wolf, K. (1998). Distance effects in surface
structures and situation models. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2,
221-246.
Rinck,
M., Hahnel, A., Bower, G.H., & Glowalla, U. (1997). The metrics of
spatial situation models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 622-637.
Rinck,
M., Williams, P., Bower, G.H., & Becker, E.S. (1996). Spatial
situation models and narrative understanding: Some generalizations
and extensions. Discourse Processes, 21, 23-55.
Shelton, A. ., & McNamara, T.P. (1997). Multiple views of
spatial memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(1), 102-106.
Shelton, A.L., & McNamara, T.P. (2001a). Systems of spatial
refererence in human memory. Cognitive Psychology, 43(4),
274-310.
Shelton, A.L., & McNamara, T.P. (2001b). Visual memories from
nonvisual experiences. Psychological Science, 12(4), 343-347.
Sholl,
M.J. (1987). Cognitive maps as orienting schemata. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13,
615-628.
Sholl,
M.J. (2000). The functional separability of self-reference and
object-to-object systems in spatial memory. In S. O'Nuallain (Ed.),
Spatial cognition: Foundations and applications (pp. 45-67).
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Sholl,
M.J., & Nolin, T.L. (1997). Orientation specificity in
representation of place. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(6), 1494-1507.
Taylor, H.A., Naylor, S. J., & Chechile, N.A. (1999). Goal-directed
influences on the representation of spatial perspective. Memory
and Cognition, 27(2), 309-319.
Taylor, H.A., & Tversky, B. (1992a). Descriptions and depictions of
environments. Memory & Cognition, 20, 483-496.
Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992b). Spatial mental models derived
from survey and route descriptions. Journal of Memory and
Language, 31, 261-282.
Taylor, H.A., & Tversky, B. (1996). Perspective in spatial
descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 371-391.
Thorndyke, P.W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial
knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive
Psychology, 14, 560-589.
Tolman, E.C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men.
Psychological Review, 55, 189-208.
Tolman, E.C., & Honzik, C.H. (1930). Insight in rats. University
of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 215-232.
Tversky, B. (1993). Cognitive maps, cognitive collages, and spatial
mental models. In A.U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial
information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (pp. 14-24).
Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Tversky, B., Morrison, J.B., & Betrancourt, M. (2002).
Animation: Can it facilitate? International Journal of
Human-Computer Studies, 57(4), 247-262.
Uttal,
D.H. (2000). Seeing the big picture: Map use and the development of
spatial cognition. Developmental Science, 3, 247-286.
Uttal,
D.H., Fisher, J.A., & Taylor, H.A. (2006). Words and maps: Developmental changes in mental models of spatial information.
Developmental Science,
9(2), 221-235.
Voyer,
D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M.P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences
in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical
variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250-270.
Waller, D., Loomis, J.M., & Haun, D.B.M. (2004). Body-based senses
enhance knowledge of directions in large-scale environments.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 157-163.
Wang,
R.F. (2004). Between reality and imagination: When is spatial
updating automatic? Perception & Psychophysics, 66(1), 68-76.
Wang,
R.F., & Brockmole, J.R. (2003a). Human navigation in nested
environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 29(3), 398-404.
Wang,
R.F., & Brockmole, J.R. (2003b). Simultaneous spatial updating in
nested environments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4),
981-986.
Wang,
R.F., & Spelke, E.S. (2000). Updating egocentric representations in
human navigation. Cognition, 77, 215-250.
Wang, R.F., & Spelke, E.S. (2002). Human spatial
representation: Insights from animals. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 6, 376-382.
Wauchope, K. (1996). Multimodal interaction with a map-based
simulation system (Technical Report No. AIC-96-019). Washington,
D.C.: Naval Research Laboratory.
Wilson, S.G., Rinck, M., McNamara, T. P., Bower, G.H.,, & Morrow, D.
G. (1993). Mental models and narrative comprehension: Some
qualifications. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 141-154.
Zwaan,
R.A., & Radvansky, G.A. (1998). Situation models in language
comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2),
162-185.