Animal Spatial
Cognition References
Abbot, H.G. (1962). The seed preferences of mice and voles in the
Northeast. Journal of Forestry, 60, 97-99.
Able, K.P. (1980). Mechanisms of orientation,
navigation and homing. In S. Gauthreaux (Ed.), Animal
migration, orientation and navigation (pp. 283-373). New
York: Academic Press.
Able, K.P. (1982). Skylight polarization patterns at dusk
influence the migratory orientation of birds. Nature, 299,
550-551.
Able, K.P. (2000). The concepts and
terminology of bird navigation. Journal of Avian Biology, 32,
174-183.
Able, K.P., & Able, M.A. (1990). Calibration of the magnetic
compass by a migratory bird by celestial rotation. Nature,
347, 378-380.
Able, K.P., & Able, M.A. (1993). Daytime
calibration of magnetic orientation in a migratory bird requires
a view of skylight polarization. Nature, 364, 523-525.
Able, K.P., & Able, M.A. (1997). Development
of sunset orientation cues in a migratory bird: no calibration
by the magnetic field. Animal Behaviour, 53, 363-368.
Aggleton, J.P., & Brown, M.W. (1999). Episodic memory, amnesia,
and the hippocampal-thalamic axis. Behavioral & Brain
Sciences, 22, 425-498.
Albert, W.S., & Golledge, R.G. (1998). The use of spatial
cognitive abilities in geographic information systems: The map
overlay operation. Transactions in GIS, 3(1), 7-21.
Alheid, G.F., de Olmos, J.S., & Beltramino, C.A. (1995).
Amygdala and extended amygdala. In G. Paxinos (Ed.), The rat
nervous system, 2nd Edition (pp. 495-578). Australia:
Academic Press.
Allen, G.L. (2000). Men and women, maps and minds: Cognitive
bases of sex-related differences in reading and interpreting
maps. In S. O'Nuallain (Ed.), Spatial cognition: Foundations
and applications (pp. 3-18). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Alvarez-Leefmans, F.J., & Gardiner-Medwin, A.R. (1975).
Influences of the septum on the hippocampal dentate area which
are unaccompanied by field potentials. The Journal of
Physiology, 249, 14P-16P.
Alyan, S., & McNaughton, B.L. (1999). Hippocampectomized rats
are capable of homing by path integration. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 113, 19-31.
Anagnostaras, S.G., Gale, G.D., & Fanselow, M.S. (2001).
Hippocampus and contextual fear conditioning: Recent
controversies and advances. Hippocampus, 11, 8-17.
Annett, L.E., McGregor, A., & Robbins, T.W. (1989). The effects
of ibotenic acid lesions of the nucleus accumbens on spatial
learning and extinction in the rat. Behavioural Brain
Research, 31, 231-242.
Arleo, A., Millan, J., & Floreano, D. (1999). Efficient learning
of variable-resolution cognitive maps for autonomous indoor
navigation. IEEE Transactions on Robotics and Automation, 15,
990-1001.
Assaf, S.Y., & Miller, J.J. (1978). Neuronal transmission in the
dentate gyrus: Role of inhibitory mechanisms. Brain Research,
151, 587-592.
Atkinson, R.C., & Shiffrin, R.M. (1968). Human memory: A
proposed system and its control processes. In K.W. Spence and
J.T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and
motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp.
89-195). New York: Academic Press.
Avraamides, M.N., Klatzky, R.L., Loomis, J.M., & Golledge, R.G.
(2004). Use of cognitive versus perceptual heading during
imagined locomotion depends on the response mode.
Psychological Science, 15(6), 403-408.
Bachelder, I.A., & Waxman, A.M. (1994). Mobile robot visual
mapping and localization: A view based neurocomputational
architecture that emulates hippocampal place learning, Neural
Networks, 7, 1083-1099.
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon
Press.
Balda, R.P. (1980). Recovery of cached seeds by a captive
Nucifraga caryocatactes. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie,
52, 331-346.
Balda, R.P. (1980). Are seed caching systems co-evolved? Acta
XVII Congr. International Ornithology. 2, 1185-1191.
Balda, R.P. (1987). Avian impacts on pinyon-juniper woodlands.
In L. Evertt (Ed.) Proceedings of the pinyon-juniper
conference (No. 7, pp. 525-533). General Technical Report
(INT-215). Reno, NV: U.S. Forest Service.
Balda, R.P. (2002). Pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus).
In A. Poole, & F. Gill (Eds.), The birds of North
America (No. 605). Philadelphia, PA: The Birds of North
America, Inc.
Balda, R.P., & Bateman, G.C. (1971). Flocking and annual cycle
of the pinyon jay. Condor, 75, 287-302.
Balda, R.P., & Bateman, G.C. (1972). The breeding biology of the
pinyon jay. Living Bird, 11, 5-42.
Balda, R.P., & Kamil, A.C. (1989). A comparative study of cache
recovery by three corvid species. Animal Behaviour, 38,
486-495.
Balda, R.P., & Kamil, A.C. (1992). Long-term spatial memory in
Clark’s nutcracker, Nucifraga columbiana. Animal Behaviour
44, 761-769.
Balda, R.P., & Kamil, A.C. (1998). The ecology and evolution of
spatial memory in corvids of the southwestern USA: The
perplexing Pinyon Jay. P. Balda, I.M. Pepperberg, & A.C. Kamil
(Eds.), Animal Cognition in Nature 7 (pp. 33-66). London,
UK: Academic Press.
Balda, R. P., & Turek, R.J. (1984). The cache-recovery system as
an example of memory capabilities in Clark's nutcracker. In
H. L. Roitblat, T.G. Beaver, & H.S. Terrace (Eds.),
Animal Cognition (pp. 513-532). London, UK: Erlbaum
Associates.
Balda, R. P., & Wiltschko, W. (1991). Caching and recovery in
scrub jays: Transfer of sun-compass directions from shaded to
sunny areas. Condor, 93, 1020-1023.
Baldocchi, D.D. (1989). Turbulent
transfer in a deciduous forest. Tree Physiology, 5,
357-377.
Barsalou, LW. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral &
Brain Sciences, 22, 577-660.
Basil, J.A. (1993). Neuroanatomical and behavioral correlates of
spatial memory in Clark's nutcrackers. Unpublished PhD.
Dissertation, University of Massachusetts.
Basil, J.A., Kamil, A.C., Balda, R.P., & Fite, K.V. Differences
in hippocampus volume among food storing corvids. Brain,
Behavior and Evolution, 47, 156-164.
Bateman, G.C., & Balda, R.P. (1973). Growth, development, and
food habits of young pinyon jays. Auk, 90, 39-61.
Beason, R.C., & Semm, P. (1987). Magnetic responses of the
trigeminal nerve system of the Bobolink Dolichonyx oryzivorus.
Neuroscience Letters, 80, 229-234.
Beason, R.C., & Semm, P. (1996). Does the avian ophthalmic nerve
carry magnetic navigational information? Journal of
Experimental Biology, 199, 1241-1244.
Beason, R.C., Wiltschko, R., & Wiltschko, W. (1997). Pigeon
homing: Effects of magnetic pulses on initial orientation.
The Auk, 114(3), 405-415.
Bednekoff, P.A. & Balda, R.P. (1996a). Social caching and
observational spatial memory in pinyon jays. Behaviour, 133,
807-826.
Bednekoff, P.A., & Balda, R.P. (1996b). Observational spatial
memory in Clark's nutcrackers and Mexican jays. Animal
Behaviour, 52, 833-839.
Bednekoff, P.A., & Balda, R.P. (1997). Clark's nutcracker
spatial memory: Many errors might not be due to forgetting.
Animal Behaviour, 54, 691-698.
Bednekoff, P.A., Balda, R.P., Kamil, A.C., & Hile, A.L. (1997).
Long term spatial memory in four seed caching corvid species.
Animal Behaviour, 53, 335-341.
Bendire (1895). Life histories of North American birds. U.S.
National Museum Bulletin, 3.
Benhamou, S., & Bovet, P.
(1992). Distinguishing between elementary orientation mechanisms
by means of path analysis. Animal Behaviour, 43,
371-377.
Benhamou, S., & Poucet, B. (1998). Landmark use by navigating
rats (Rattus norvegicus): Contrasting geometric and
featural information. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
112, 317-322.
Benhamou, S., Poucet, B., & Bovet, P.
(1995). A model for place navigation in mammals. Journal of
Theoretical Biology, 173, 163-178.
Benkman, C.W., Balda, R.P., & Smith, C.C. (1984).Adaptations for
seed dispersal and the compromises due to seed predation in
limber pine. Ecology 65: 632-642.
Bennett, A.T.D. (1993). Spatial memory in a food storing corvid.
I. Near tall landmarks are primarily used. Journal of
Comparative Physiology A, 173, 193-207.
Bennett, A.T.D. (1996). Do animals have cognitive maps? The
Journal of Experimental Biology, 1999, 219-224.
Benvenuti, S., & Wallraff, H.G. (1985). Pigeon navigation: Site
simulation by means of atmospheric odours. Journal of
Comparative Physiology A, 156, 737-746.
Berlyne, D.E. (1950). Novelty and curiosity as determinants of
exploratory behavior. British Journal of Psychology, 41,
68-80.
Berthold, P. (1990). Spatiotemporal programs and genetics of
orientation. Experientia, 46, 363-371.
Berthold, P. (1991). Genetic control of migratory behaviour in
birds. TREE, 6, 254-257.
Berthold, P. (2003). Genetic basis and evolutionary aspects of
bird migration. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 33,
175-229.
Berthold, P., & Querner, U. (1981) Genetic basis of migratory
behavior in European warblers. Science, 212, 77-79.
Best, P.J., & White, A.M. (1999). Placing hippocampal
single-unit studies in a historical context. Hippocampus, 9,
346-351.
Best, P.J., White, A.M., & Minai, A. (2001). Spatial processing
in the brain: The activity of hippocampal place cells. Annual
Review of Neuroscience, 24, 459-486.
Biegler, R. (1996). Short and medium range
navigation and its relationship to cognitive mapping and
associative learning. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of
Edinburgh, Edinburgh, Scotland.
Biegler, R. (2000). Possible uses of path
integration in animal navigation. Animal Learning & Behavior,
28(3), 257-277.
Biegler, R. (2003). Reading cognitive and
other maps: How to avoid getting buried in thought. In K. J.
Jeffery (Ed.), The neurobiology of spatial behaviour.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Biegler, R., McGregor, A., Krebs, J.R., & Healy, S.D. (2001). A
larger hippocampus is associated with longer-lasting spatial
memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of America, 98, 6941-6944.
Bilge, A.R., & Taylor, H.A. (2006). Learning nested environment
from maps: Is spatial updating simultaneous? Unpublished
data. Tufts University.
Bilkey, D.K., & Goddard, G.V. (1985). Medial septal facilitation
of hippocampal granule cell activity is mediated by inhibition
of inhibitory interneurones. Brain Research, 361, 99-106.
Bingman, V.P. (1983). Magnetic field orientation of migratory
Savannah sparrows with different first summer experience.
Behaviour, 87, 43-53.
Bingman, V.P., Bagnoli, P., Ioalé, P., & Casini,
G. (1984). Homing behavior of pigeons after telencephalic
ablations. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 24, 94-106.
Bingman, V.P., Budzynski, C.A., & Voggenhuber, A. (2003).
Migratory systems as adaptive responses to spatial and temporal
variability in orientation stimuli. In P. Berthold, E. Gwinner,
& E. Sonnenschein (Eds.), Avian migration (pp. 457-469).
Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg-New York.
Bingman, V.P., & Cheng, K. (2005). Mechanisms of animal global
navigation: Comparative perspectives and enduring challenges.
Ethology, Ecology & Evolution, 17, 295-318.
Bingman, V.P., Gagliardo, A., & Ioalè, P. (1996). Hippocampal
participation in the sun compass orientation of phase-shifted
homing pigeons. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 179,
695-702.
Bingman, V.P., Hough II, G.E., Kahn, M.C., & Siegel, J.J.
(2003). The homing pigeon hippocampus and space: In search of
adaptive specialization. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 62,
117-127.
Bingman, V.P., Ioalé, P., Casini, G., & Bagnoli, P. (1990). The
avian hippocampus: Evidence for a role in the development of the
homing pigeon navigational map. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104,
906-911.
Bingman, V.P., & Jones, T.J. (1994). Sun compass based spatial
learning impaired in homing pigeons with hippocampal lesions.
Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 6687-6694.
Biro, D., Meade, J., & Guilford, T. (2004). Familiar route
loyalty implies visual pilotage in the homing pigeon.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 101, 17440-17443.
Blackford-Cook, S. (1969). Experiments on
homing in the limpet Siphonaria normalis. Animal
Behaviour, 17, 679-682.
Bletz, H., Weindler, P., Wiltschko, R., & Wiltschko, W.
(1996). The magnetic field as reference for the innate migratory
direction in blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla).
Naturwissenschaften, 83,
430-432.
Blum, K.I., & Abbott, L.R. (1996). A model of spatial map
formation in the hippocampus of the rat. Neural Computation,
8, 85-93.
Bock, W.J., Balda, R.P., & Vander Wall, S.B. (1973). Morphology
of the sublingual pouch and tongue musculature in Clark's
nutcrackers. Auk, 90, 491-519.
Boles, L.C., & Lohmann, K.J. (2003). True navigation and
magnetic maps in spiny lobsters. Nature, 421, 60-63.
Bostock, E., Muller, R.U., & Kubie, J.L. (1991).
Experience-dependent modifications of hippocampal place cell
firing. Hippocampus, 1, 193-205.
Bower, G.H., & Morrow, D.G. (1990). Mental models in narrative
comprehension. Science, 247(4938), 44-48.
Bower, G.H., & Rinck, M. (2001). Selecting one among many
referents in spatial situation models. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27,
81-98.
Braithwaite, V.A., & Girvan, J.R. (2003). Use of water flow
direction to provide spatial information in a small-scale
orientation task. Journal of Fish Biology, 63,
74-83.
Breese, C.R., Hampson, R.E., & Deadwyler, S.A. (1989).
Hippocampal place cells: Stereotypy and plasticity. Journal
of Neuroscience, 9, 1097-1111.
Brines, M.L., & Gould, J.L. (1982). Skylight polarization
patterns and animal orientation. Journal of Experimental
Biology, 96, 69-91.
Brodbeck, D.R. (1994). Memory for spatial and local cues - A
comparison of a storing and a non-storing species. Animal
Learning & Behavior, 22, 119-133.
Brodbeck, D.R. & Shettleworth, S.J. (1995). Matching location
and color of a compound stimulus - comparison of a food-storing
and a nonstoring bird species. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 64-77.
van den Broek, P., Rapp, D.N., & Kendeou, P. (2005). Integrating
memory-based and constructionist processes in accounts of
reading comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39, 299-316.
van den Broek, P., Risden, K., Fletcher, C.R., & Thurlow, R.
(1996). A "landscape" view of reading: Fluctuating patterns of
activation and the construction of a stable memory
representation. In B. K. Britton & A. C. Graesser (Eds.),
Models of understanding text (pp. 165-187). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Brown, J.L. (1994). Mexican jay (Aphelocoma ultramarine).
In A. Poole, & F. Gill (Eds.), The birds of North America,
no.118. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, PA, and
American Ornithologists’ Union, Washington D. C.
Brown, M.A., & Sharp, P.E. (1995). Simulation
of spatial learning in the Morris water maze by a neural network
model of the hippocampus and nucleus accumbens. Hippocampus,
5(3), 171-188.
Brown, M.F., DiGello, E., Milewski, M., Wilson, M., & Kozak, M.
(2000). Spatial pattern learning in rats: Conditional control by
two patterns. Animal Learning & Behavior, 28, 278-287.
Brown, M.F., & Huggins, C.K. (1993). Maze-arm length affects a
choice criterion in the radial-arm maze. Animal Learning &
Behavior, 21, 68-72.
Brown, M.F., & Lesniak-Karpiak, K.B. (1993). Choice criterion
effects in the radial-arm maze: Maze-arm incline and brightness.
Learning and Motivation, 24, 23-39.
Brown, M.F., & Terrinoni, M. (1996). Control of choice by the
spatial configuration of goals. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 438-446.
Brown, M.F., & Wintersteen, J. (2004). Spatial pattern learning
and spatial working memory. Learning & Behavior, 34,
391-400.
Brown, M.F., Yang, S.Y., & DiGian, K.A. (2002). No evidence for
overshadowing or facilitation of spatial pattern learning by
visual cues. Animal Learning & Behavior, 30, 363-375.
Brown, M.F., Zeiler, C., & John, A. (2001). Spatial pattern
learning in rats: Control by an iterative pattern. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27,
407-416.
Brown, S.F., & Mellgren, R.L.
(1994). Distinction between places and paths in rats' spatial
representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 20(1), 20-31.
Brunye, T.T., Taylor, H.A., & Rapp, D.N. (2005). Putting
things together: Memory mechanisms underlying the benefits of
multimedia learning. Paper presented at the 46th Annual
Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Toronto, Ontario.
Budzynski, C.A., Dyer, F.C., & Bingman, V.P. (2000). Partial
experience with the arc of the sun is sufficient for all day sun
compass orientation in young homing pigeons, Columbia livia. Journal
of Experimental Biology, 203, 2341 - 2348.
Bures, J., Buresova, O., & Nerad, L. (1992).
Can rats solve a simple version of the travelling salesman
problem? Behavioural Brain Research, 52, 133-142.
Buresova, O., Homuta, L., Krekule, I., &
Bures, J. (1988). Does non-directional signalization of target
distance contribute to navigation in the Morris water maze?
Behavioral and Neural Biology, 49(2), 240-248.
Burgess, N., Jackson, A., Hartley, T., & O’Keefe, J. (2000).
Predictions derived from modeling the hippocampal role in
navigation. Biological Cybernetics, 83, 301-312.
Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. (2003). Neural representations in
human spatial memory. Trends in Cognitive Science,
7, 517-519.
Burgess, N., Recce, M., & O'Keefe, J.
(1994). A model of hippocampal function. Neural Networks, 7,
1065-1081.
Burnell, K.L., & Tomback, D.F. (1985). Steller's jays steal gray
jay caches: Field and laboratory observations. Auk, 102,
417-491.
Burt, T., Holland, R., & Guilford, T. (1997). Further
evidence for visual landmarks involvement in the pigeons'
familiar area map. Animal Behavior, 53, 1203-1209.
Buzsaki, G. (2002). Theta oscillations in the hippocampus.
Neuron, 33, 325-340.
Cain, P., & Malwal, S. (2002). Landmark use and development of
navigation behaviour in the weakly electric fish Gnathonemus
petersii (Mormyridae; Teleostei). Journal of Experimental
Biology, 205, 3915-3923.
Canfield, J.G., & Mizumori, S.J. (2004). Methods for chronic
neural recording in the telencephalon of freely behaving fish.
Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 133, 127-134.
Car, A. (1998). Hierarchical spatial reasoning: A geocomputation
method. Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on
GeoComputation, 17-19 September, Bristol.
Cardinal, R.N., Winstanley, C.A., Robbins, T.W., Everitt, B.J.
(2004). Limbic corticostriatal systems and delayed
reinforcement. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
1021, 33-50.
Carlson-Radvansky, L.A., & Radvansky, G.A. (1996). The influence
of functional relations on spatial term selection.
Psychological Science, 7(1), 56-60.
Cartwright, B.A., & Collett, T.S. (1982). How
honey bees use landmarks to guide their return to food.
Nature, 295, 560-564.
Cartwright, B.A, & Collett, T.S.
(1983). Landmark learning in bees: Experiments and models.
Journal of Comparative Physiology, 151, 521-543.
Chamizo, V.D. (2003). Acquisition of
knowledge about spatial location: Assessing the generality of
the mechanism of learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 56B, 102-113.
Charnov, E.L. (1976). Optimal foraging: The
marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9,
129-136.
Chen, B. (2000). Differential spatial memory by male and female
pinyon jays. Unpublished M.S. thesis. Northern Arizona
University, Flagstaff.
Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module
in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition, 23,
149-178.
Cheng, K. (1988). Some psychophysics of the
pigeon‘s use of landmarks. Journal of Comparative Physiology
A. 162, 815-826.
Cheng, K. (1990). More psychophysics of the
pigeon‘s use of landmarks. Journal of Comparative Physiology
A. 166, 857-863.
Cheng, K. (2000). How honeybees find a place:
Lessons from a simple mind. Animal Learning & Behavior,
28(1), 1-15.
Cheng, K. (2005a). Reflections on geometry and navigation.
Connection Science,
17, 5-21.
Cheng, K. (2005b). Goldfish matching
geometric and featural cues: A re-interpretation of some of the
data of Vargas et al. (2004). Journal of Comparative
Psychology,
119, 455-457.
Cheng, K., Collett, T.S., Pickhard, A., & Wehner, R. (1987). The
use of visual landmarks by honeybees: Bees weight landmarks
according to their distance from the goal. Journal of
Comparative Physiology A, 161, 469-475.
Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C.R. (1984). Testing the geometric power
of a spatial representation. In H.L. Roitblat, H.S. Terrace, &
T.G. Bever (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 409-423).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cheng, K., & Gallistel, C.R. (2005). Shape parameters explain
data from spatial transformations: Comment on Pearce et al.
(2004) and Tommasi, & Polli (2004). Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 254-259.
Cheng, K., & Newcombe, N.S. (2005). Is there a geometric module
for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 1-23.
Cheng, K., & Sherry, D.F. (1992). Landmark-based spatial memory
in birds (Parus atricapillus and Columbia livia):
The use of edges and distances to represent spatial positions.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior
Processes, 106, 331-341.
Cheng, K., & Spetch, M.L. (1998). Mechanisms
of landmarks use in mammals and birds. In S. D. Healy (Ed.),
Spatial representation in animals. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Chew, G.L., Sutherland, R.J., & Whishaw, I.Q.
(1989). Latent learning does not produce instantaneous transfer
of place navigation: A rejoinder to Keith and McVety.
Psychobiology, 17, 207-209.
Chittka, L., & Geiger, K. (1995a). The
influences of landmarks on distance estimation of honey bees.
Animal Behaviour, 50, 23-31.
Chittka, L., & Geiger, K. (1995b). Honeybee
long-distance orientation in a controlled environment. Ethology,
99, 117-126.
Chittka, L., & Kunze, J. (1995). The
significance of landmarks for path integration in homing
honeybee foragers. Naturwissenschaften, 82, 341-343.
Chown, E., Kaplan, S., & Kortenkamp, D. (1995). Prototypes,
location, and associative networks (PLAN): Toward a unified
theory of cognitive mapping. Cognitive Science, 19, 1-51.
Christensen, K.M., & Whitham, T.G. (1993). Impact of insect
herbivores on competition between birds and mammals for pinyon
pine seeds. Ecology, 74, 2270-2278.
Clark, L., & Shah, P.S. (1992). Information content of odor
plumes: What do Leach's Storm Petrels know? In R.L. Doty & D.
Muller-Schwarze (Eds.), Chemical Signals in Vertebrates (Vol.
6, pp. 421-427). New York: Plenum Press.
Clayton, N.S., & Krebs, J.R. (1994). Memory
for spatial and object-specific cues in food-storing and
non-storing birds. Journal of Comparative Physiology A:
Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology, 174,
371-379.
Clifford, K.T., Gross, L., Johnson, K.,
Martin, K. J., Shaheen, N., & Harrington, M.A.
(2003). Slime-trail tracking in the predatory snail,
Euglandina rosea. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117(5),
1086-1095.
Cochran, W.W., Mouritsen, H., & Wikelski, M.
(2004). Migrating songbirds recalibrate their magnetic compass
daily from twilight cues. Science, 304, 405-408.
Cohen, N.J., & Eichenbaum, H. (1994).
Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Cohen, P., Johnston, M., McGee, D., Oviatt,
S., Clow, J., & Smith, I. (1998). The efficiency of
multimodal interaction: A case study. Paper presented at
the International Conference on Spoken Language Processes,
Australia.
Colby, C.L., & Goldberg, M.E. (1999). Space
and attention in parietal cortex. Annu Reviews in the
Neurosciencesence, 22, 319-349.
Collett, M., & Collett, T.S. (2000). How do
insects use path integration for their navigation? Biological
Cybernetics, 83, 245-259.
Collett, M., Collett, T.S., Bisch, S., &
Wehner, R. (1998). Local and global vectors in desert ant
navigation. Nature, 394, 269-272.
Collett, T.S. (1982). Do toads plan routes?
A study of detour behaviour in Bufo viridis. Journal
of Comparative Physiology, 146, 261-271.
Collett, T.S. (1995). Making learning easy:
The acquisition of visual information during the orientation
flight of social wasps. Journal of Comparative Physiology A,
177, 737-747.
Collett, T.S. (1996). Insect navigation en
route to the goal: Multiple strategies for the use of landmarks.
Journal of Experimental Biology, 199(1), 227-235.
Collett, T.S., & Baron, J. (1994). Biological
compasses and the coordinate frame of landmark memories in
honeybees. Nature, 368, 137-140.
Collett, T.S., Dillmann, E., Giger, A., &
Wehner, R. (1992). Visual landmarks and route following in
desert ants. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 170,
435-442.
Collett, T.S., & Harkness, L.I.K.
(1982). Depth vision in animals. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale.
& R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Cook, A. (1977). Mucus trail following by the
slug Limax grossui Lupu. Animal Behaviour, 25,
774-781.
Cook, A., Bamford, O.S., Freeman, J.D.B., &
Teidemann, D.J. (1969). A study of the homing habit of the
limpet. Animal Behaviour, 17, 330-339.
Courtillot, V., Hulot, G., Alexandrescu, M.,
le Mouël, J.L., & Kirschvink, J.L. (1997). Sensitivity and
evolution of sea-turtle magnetoreception: Observations,
modelling and constraints from geomagnetic secular variation.
Terra Nova, 9, 203-207.
Cramer, A.E., & Gallistel, C.R.
(1997). Vervet monkeys as travelling salesmen. Nature,
387, 464.
Cristol, D.A., Reynolds, E.B., Leclerc, J.E.,
Donner, A.H., Farabaugh, C.S., & Ziegenfus, C.W.S. (2003).
Migratory dark-eyed juncos, Junco hyemalis, have better
spatial memory and denser hippocampal neurons than nonmigratory
conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 66, 317-328.
Crocq, C. (1978). Ecologie du Casse-noix
(Nucifraga caryocatactes L.) dans les Alpes Francaises du
sud: Ses relations avec L'Arolle (Pinus cembra L.).
Unpublished PhD, L'Univerite d'Aix-Marseille, Marseille, France.
Crystal, J.D. & Shettleworth, S.J. (1994).
Spatial list learning in black-capped chickadees. Animal
Learning & Behavior, 22, 77-83.
Dabbs, J.M., Chang, E.L., Strong, R.A., &
Milun, R. (1998). Spatial ability, navigation strategy, and
geographic knowledge among men and women. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 19, 89-98.
Dahl, D., & Winson, J. (1985). Action of
norepinephrine in the dentate gyrus. I. Stimulation of locus
coeruleus. Experimental Brain Research, 59,
491-496.
Dallal, N.L., & Meck, W.H. (1990).
Hierarchical structures: Chunking by food type facilitates
spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 16, 69-84.
Dawkins, M.S., Guilford, T., Braithwaite,
V.A., & Krebs, J.R. (1996). Discrimination and recognition of
photographs of places by homing pigeons. Behavioural
Processes, 36, 27-38.
Deutsch, J.A. (1960). The structural
basis of behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Deutschlander, M.E., Borland, S.C., &
Phillips J.B. (1999). Extraocular magnetic compass in newts. Nature,
400, 324-325.
Devan, B.D., & White, N.M. (1999). Parallel
information processing in the dorsal striatum: Relation to hippocampal function. Journal of Neuroscience, 19,
2789-2798.
deVega, M. (1995). Backward updating of
mental models during continuous reading of narratives.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 21, 373-385.
Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary animal
learning theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Diego-Rasilla, F.J., Luengo, R.M., & Phillips,
J.B. (2005). Magnetic compass mediates nocturnal homing by the
alpine newt, Triturus alpestris. Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology, 58,
361-365.
DiGello, E., Brown, M.F., & Affuso, J.
(2002). Negative information: Both presence and absence of
spatial pattern elements guide rats' spatial choices.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 706-713.
Dijkstra, E.W. (1959). A note on two problems
in connection with graphs. Numeriske Mathematik, 1,
269-271.
Diwadkar, V.A., & McNamara, T.P. (1997).
Viewpoint dependence in scene recognition. Psychological
Science, 8(4), 302-307.
Donnart, J.Y., & Meyer, J.A. (1996).
Hierarchical-map building and self-positioning with MonaLysa.
Adaptive Behavior, 5(1) 29-74.
Donoghue, J.P., & Parham, C. (1983). Afferent
connections of the lateral agranular field of the rat motor
cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 217, 390-404.
Dudchenko, P. (2003). The head direction
system and navigation. In K.J. Jeffery (Ed.), The
neurobiology of spatial behaviour (pp. 173-186). Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Dudek, G., & Jenkin, M. (2000).
Computational principles of mobile robotics. Cambridge
University Press.
Duff, S.J., & Hampson, E. (2001). A sex
difference on a novel spatial working memory task in humans.
Brain and Cognition, 47, 470-493.
Eason. P.K., Cobbs, G.A., & Trinca, K.G.
(1999). The use of landmarks to define territorial boundaries. Animal
Behaviour, 58(1), 85-91.
Easton, R.D., & Sholl, M.J. (1995).
Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of
spatial knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 483-500.
Egerton, S., Callaghan, V., & Chernett, P.
(2000). A biologically inspired mapping model for autonomous
mobile robots. In M. Mohammadin (Ed.), New frontiers in
computational intelligence and its applications (pp. 20-29).
Amsterdam: IOS Press.
Ehrlich, K., & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1982).
Spatial descriptions and referential continuity. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(3), 296-306.
Eichenbaum, H., & Cohen, N.J. (2001). From
conditioning to conscious recollection: Memory systems of the
brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
Eichenbaum, H., Kuperstein, M., Fagan, A., &
Nagode, J. (1987). Cue-sampling and goal-approach correlates of
hippocampal unit activity in rats performing an
odor-discrimination task. Journal of Neuroscience, 7,
716-732.
Ekstrom, A.D., Kahana, M.J., Caplan, J.B.,
Fields, T.A., Isham, E.A., Newman, E.L., et al. (2003). Cellular
networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature,
425, 184-187.
Emlen, S.T. (1967). Migratory orientation in
the Indigo Bunting, Passerina cyanea. Part II. Mechanisms
of celestial orientation. Auk, 84, 463-489.
Emlen, S.T. (1970). Celestial rotation: Its
importance in the development of migratory orientation.
Science, 170, 1198-1201.
Emlen, S.T. (1975). Migration: Orientation
and navigation. In D.S. Farner & J.R. King (Eds.), Avian
biology (Vol. 5, pp. 129-219). New York: Academic Press.
Epstein, R., DeYoe, E.A., Press, D.Z., Rosen,
A.C., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). Neuropsychological evidence for a
topographical learning mechanism in parahippocampal cortex.
Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 481-508.
Epstein, R., Graham, K.S., & Downing, P.E.
(2003). Viewpoint-specific scene representations in human
parahippocampal cortex. Neuron, 37, 865-876.
Epstein, R., Harris, A., Stanley, D., &
Kanwisher, N. (1999). The parahippocampal place area:
Recognition, navigation, or encoding? Neuron, 23,
115-125.
Epstein, R., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). A
cortical representation of the local visual environment.
Nature, 392, 598-601.
Etienne, A.S., & Jeffery, K.J. (2004). Path
integration in mammals. Hippocampus, 14(2),
180-192.
Etienne, A.S., Teroni, E., Hurni, C., &
Portenier, V. (1990). The effect of a single light cue on homing
behaviour of the golden hamster. Animal Behaviour, 39,
17-41.
Farrell, M.J., & Robertson, I.H. (1998).
Mental rotation and automatic updating of body-centered spatial
relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 24(1), 227-233.
Ferbinteanu, J., & Shapiro, M.L. (2003).
Prospective and retrospective memory coding in the hippocampus.
Neuron, 40, 1227-1239.
Fischer, J.H., Freake, M.J., Borland, S.C., &
Phillips, J.B. (2001). Evidence for the use of a magnetic map by
an amphibian. Animal Behavior, 62, 1-10.
Fischer, J., Munro, U., & Phillips, J.B.
(2003). Magnetic navigation by an avian migrant? In P. Berthold,
E. Gwinner, & E. Sonnenschein (Eds.), Avian migration
(pp. 423-432). Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg-New York.
Fitzgerald, T.D. (2003). Role of trail
pheromone in foraging and processionary behaviour of pine
processionary caterpillars Thaumetopoea pityocampa. Journal
of Chemical Ecology, 29(3), 513-532.
Flaherty, A.W., & Graybiel, A.M. (1993). Two
input systems for body representations in the primate striatal
matrix: Experimental evidence in the squirrel monkey. Journal
of Neuroscience, 13, 1120-1137.
Fleissner, G., Holtkamp-Rötzler, E., Hanzlik,
M., Winklhofer, M., Fleissner, G., Petersen, N., & Wiltschko, W.
(2003). Ultrastructural analysis of a putative magnetoreceptor
in the beak of homing pigeons. Journal of Comparative
Neurology, 458, 350-360.
Floresco, S.B., Seamans, J.K., & Phillips,
A.G. (1996). Differential effects of lidocaine infusions into
the ventral CA1/subiculum or the nucleus accumbens on the
acquisition and retention of spatial information. Behavioural
Brain Research, 8, 163-171.
Foa, A., Bagnoli, P., & Giongo, F.
(1986). Homing pigeons subjected to section of the anterior
commissure can build up two olfactory maps in the deflector
lofts. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 159,
465-472.
Fodor, J.A. (1983). The modularity of mind.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Foo, P., Wareen, W.H., Duchon, A., & Tarr,
M.J. (2005). Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map?
Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 31(2), 195-215.
Fountain, S.B., & Rowan, J.D. (1995). Coding
for hierarchical versus linear pattern structure in rats and
humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 21, 187-202.
Fox, S.E., & Ranck, J.B., Jr. (1981).
Electrophysiological characteristics of hippocampal
complex-spike cells and theta cells. Experimental Brain
Research, 41, 399-410.
Freake, M.J., & Phillips, J.B. (2005).
Light-dependent shift in bullfrog tadpole magnetic compass
orientation: Evidence for a common magnetoreception mechanism
in anuran and urodele amphibians. Ethology, 111,
241-254.
Freeman, J.H., Jr., Cuppernell, C., Flannery,
K., & Gabriel, M. (1996) Limbic thalamic, cingulate cortical and
hippocampal neuronal correlates of discriminative approach
learning in rabbits. Behavioural Brain Research, 80,
123-136.
Freeman, J.H., Jr., Weible, A., Rossi, J., &
Gabriel, M. (1997). Lesions of the entorhinal cortex disrupt
behavioral and neuronal responses to context change during
extinction of discriminative avoidance behavior. Experimental
Brain Research, 115, 445-457.
Freund, T.F., & Antal, M. (1988).
GABA-containing neurons in the septum control inhibitory
interneurons in the hippocampus. Nature, 336,
170-173.
Freund, T.F., & Buzsaki, G. (1996).
Interneurons of the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 6, 347-470.
von Frisch, K. (1967). The dance language
and orientation of bees. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Fritsch, D.S., Pizer, S.M., Morse, B.S.,
Eberly, D.H., & Liu, A. (1994). The multiscale medial axis and
its applications in image registration. Pattern Recognition
Letters, 15, 445-452.
Gagliardo, A., Ioalé, P., & Bingman, V.P.
(1999). Homing in pigeons: The role of the hippocampal formation
in the representation of landmarks used for navigation.
Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 311-315.
Gagliardo, A., Ioale, P., Odetti, F., Kahn,
M.C., & Bingman, V.P. (2004). Hippocampal lesions do not
dirupt navigational map retention in homing pigeons under
conditions when amp acquisition is hippocampal dependent.
Behavioural Brain Research, 153, 35-42.
Gagliardo, A., Mazzotto, M., & Bingman, V.
(1997). Piriform cortex ablations block navigational map
learning in homing pigeons. Behavioural Brain Research, 86,
143-148.
Gagliardo, A., Odetti, F., & Ioalé, P.
(2001). Relevance of visual cues for orientation at familiar
sites by homing pigeons: An experiment in a circular arena.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 268,
2065-2070.
Gagliardo, A., Odetti, F., Ioalé, P.,
Bingman, V.P., Tuttle, S., & Vallortigara, G. (2002). Bilateral
participation of the hippocampus in familiar landmark navigation
by homing pigeons. Behavioural Brain Research, 136,
201-209.
Gal, G., Joel, D., Gusak, O., Feldon, J., &
Weiner, I. (1997). The effects of electrolytic lesion to the
shell subterritory of the nucleus accumbens on delayed
non-matching-to-sample and four-arm baited eight-arm radial-maze
tasks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 92-103.
Galea, L.A.M., Kavaliers, M., Ossenkopp,
K.P., Innes, D., & Hargreaves, E.L. (1994a). Sexually dimorphic
spatial learning varies seasonally in two populations of deer
mice. Brain Research, 635, 18-26.
Galea, L.A.M., & McEwen, B.S. (1999). Sex and
seasonal differences in the rate of cell proliferation in the
dentate gyrus of adult wild meadow voles. Neuroscience,
89, 955-964.
Galea, L.A.M., Saksida, L., Kavaliers, M., &
Ossenkopp, K.P. (1994b). Naloxone facilitates spatial learning
in a water-maze task in female, but not male, adult non-breeding
meadow voles. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior,
47, 265-271.
Gallistel, C.R. (1990). The organization
of learning. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
Ganzhorn, J.U. (1990). Towards the map of the
homing pigeon. Animal Behaviour, 40, 65-78.
Ganzhorn, J.U. (1992). Geographical patterns
in the initial orientation of homing pigeons in upstate New
York. Animal Behavior, 44, 931-941.
Ganzhorn, J.U. (1995). Patterns of air
pollution as model for the physical basis for olfactory
navigation in pigeon homing. Journal of Ornithology, 136,
159-165.
Ganzhorn, J.U., & Schmidt-Koenig, K.
(1990). Initial orientation of homing pigeons around Tübingen.
In R. van der Elzen, K.L. Schuchmann, & K. Schmidt-Koenig
(Eds.), Current topics in avian biology (pp. 305-309).
Deutsche Ornithologen Gesselschaft, Bonn.
Garrison, J.S.E., & Gass, C.L. (1999).
Response of a traplining hummingbird to changes in nectar
availability. Behavioral Ecology, 10, 714-725.
Gass, C.L., & Garrison, J.S.E. (1999). Energy
regulation by traplining hummingbirds. Functional Ecology,
13, 438-492.
Gattis, M. (2001). Spatial schemas and
abstract thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gauvain, M. (2001). Cultural tools, social
interaction and the development of thinking. Human
Development, 44, 126-143.
Gehlbach, F. R., Watkins, J. F., & Kroll, J.
C. (1971). Pheromone trail-following studies of typhlopid,
leptotyphlopid and colubrid snakes. Behaviour, 40,
282-294.
Gemmell, C., & O'Mara, S.M. (1999). Medial
prefrontal cortex lesions cause deficits in a variable-goal
location task but not in object exploration. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 113, 465-474.
Gentner, D., & Stevens, A.L. (1983).
Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gernsbacher, M.A. (1990). Language
comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Gibson, B.M., & Shettleworth, S.J. (2003).
Competition among spatial cues in a naturalistic food-carrying
task. Learning & Behavior, 31, 143-159.
Gilmartin, P. (1986). Maps, mental imagery,
and gender in the recall of geographic information. The
American Cartographer, 13, 335-344.
Girvan, J.R. & Braithwaite, V.A. (1998).
Population differences in spatial learning in three-spined
sticklebacks. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London,
Series B, 265, 913-918.
Girvan, J.R., & Braithwaite, V.A. (2000).
Orientation behaviour in sticklebacks: Modified by experience or
population specific? Behaviour, 137, 833-843.
Giuntoli, M., & Mewaldt, L.R. (1978). Stomach
contents of Clark's nutcrackers collected in western Montana.
Auk, 95, 595-598.
Givon, T. (1992). The grammar of referential
coherence as mental processing instructions. Linguistics.
Special topic: From grammar to discourse, 30(1), 5-55.
Glasgow, J., Narayanan, N.H., &
Chandrasekaran, B. (Eds.). (1995). Diagrammatic reasoning:
Cognitive and computational perspectives. Menlo Park, CA:
AAAI Press.
Gold, P.E. (2003). Acetylcholine modulation
of neural systems involved in learning and memory.
Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 80,194-210.
Golledge, R.G., & Spector, N.A. (1978).
Comprehending the urban environment: Theory and practice.
Geographical Analysis, 14, 305-325.
Golob, E.J., Stackman, R.W., Wong, A.C., &
Taube, J.S. (2001). On the behavioral significance of head
direction cells: Neural and behavioral dynamics during spatial
memory tasks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115,
285-304.
Gonor, J.J. (1967). Predator-prey reactions
between two marine Prosobranch gastropods. The Veliger, 7,
228-232.
Goodyear, A.J., & Kamil, A.C. (2004). Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) and
the effects of goal‑landmark distance on overshadowing. Journal
of Comparative Psychology, 118, 258-264.
Gould, J.L. (1982).
The map sense of pigeons. Nature, 296, 205-211.
Gould, J.L. (1985). Are animal maps magnetic?
In J.L Kirschvink, D.S. Jones, & B.J. McFadden (Eds), Magnetite
biomineralization and magnetoreception in organisms: A new
biomagnetism (pp. 257-268). New York and London: Plenum Press.
Gould-Beierle, K.L., & Kamil, A.C. (1996).
The use of local and global cues by Clark's nutcrackers,
Nucifraga columbiana. Animal Behaviour, 52, 519-528.
Gould-Beierle, K.L., & Kamil, A.C. (1998).
Use of landmarks in three species of food-storing corvids.
Ethology, 104, 361-378.
Gould-Beierle, K.L., & Kamil, A.C. (1999).
The role of proximity in landmark use by Clark's nutcrackers.
Animal Behaviour, 58, 477-488.
Gouteux, S., & Spelke, E.S. (2001).
Children's use of geometry and landmarks to reorient in an open
space. Cognition, 81, 119-148.
Gouteux, S., Thinus-Blanc, C., & Vauclair, J.
(2001). Rhesus monkeys use geometric and non-geometric
information during a reorientation task. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 505-519.
Graham, M., Good, M.A.,
McGregor, A., & Pearce, J.M. (2006). Spatial learning based on
the shape of the environment is influenced by properties of the
objects forming the shape. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 44-59.
Graham, S.M., Joshi, A., & Pizlo, Z. (2000).
The travelling salesman problem: A hierarchical model. Memory
and Cognition. 28(7), 1191-1204.
Granon, S., Save, E., Buhot, M.C., & Poucet,
B. (1996). Effortful information processing in a spontaneous
spatial situation by rats with medial prefrontal lesions.
Behavioural Brain Research, 78, 147-154.
Grant, D., Anderson, O., & Twitty, V.
(1968). Homing orientation by olfaction in newts Taricha
rivularis. Science, 160, 1354-1355.
Grant, K.A. (1966). A hypothesis concerning
the prevalence of red coloration in California hummingbird
flowers. American Naturalist, 100, 85-97.
Gray, E.R., Bloomfield,
L.L., Ferrey, A., Spetch, M.L., & Sturdy, C.B. (2005). Spatial
encoding in mountain chickadees: Features overshadow geometry.
Biology Letters, 1, 314-317.
Griffin, D.R. (1952). Bird navigation.
Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 27,
359-400.
van Groen, T., & Wyss, J.M. (1990). Connections of the
retrosplenial granular a cortex in the rat. Journal of
Comparative Neurology, 300, 593-606.
Guilford, T., Gagliardo, A., Chappell, J.,
Bonadonna, F., Burt, T., & Holland, R. (1998). Homing pigeons
use olfactory cues for navigation in England. Journal of
Experimental Biology, 201, 895-900.
Guilford, T., Roberts, S., Biro, D., & Rezek,
L. (2004). Positional entropy during pigeon homing II:
Navigational interpretation of Bayesian latent state models.
Journal of Theoretical Biology, 227, 25-38.
Güntürkün, O. (1997) Avian visual
lateralization: A review. NeuroReport, 8(6), 3-11.
Gwinner, E., Wiltschko, W. (1978).
Endogenously controlled changes in the migratory direction of
garden warbler, Sylvia borin. Journal of Comparative
Physiology, 125, 267-273.
Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Moser, M. B., & Moser, E. I.
(2005). Microstructure of a spatial map in the
entorhinal cortex. Nature, 436, 801-806.
Hakala, C. (1999). Accessibility of spatial
information in a situation model. Discourse Processes, 27,
261-280.
Hampton, R.R., Healy, S.D., Shettleworth,
S.J., & Kamil, A.C. (2002). 'Neuroecologists' are not made of
straw. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 6-7.
Hampton, R.R., & Shettleworth, S.J. (1996a).
Hippocampal lesions impair memory for location but not color in
passerine birds. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110,
831-835.
Hampton, R.R., & Shettleworth, S.J. (1996b).
Hippocampus and memory in a food-storing and in a nonstoring
bird species. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110,
946-964.
Hartley, T., Burgess, N., Lever, C., Cacucci,
F., & O’Keefe, J. (2000). Modeling place fields in terms of the
cortical inputs to the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 10,
369-379.
Hartley, T., Maguire, E.A., Spiers, H.J., &
Burgess, N. (2003). The well-worn route and the path less
travelled: Distinct neural bases of route following and way
finding in humans. Neuron, 37, 877-888.
Hartley, T., Trinkler, I., & Burgess, N.
(2004). Geometric determinants of human spatial memory.
Cognition, 94, 39-75.
Hartmann, G., & Wehner, R. (1995). The ant's
path integration system: A neural architecture. Biological
Cybernetics, 73, 483–493.
Harzi, M.M., & Jarrard, L.E. (1992). Effects
of medial and lateral septal lesions on acquisition of a place
and cue radial maze task. Behavioral Brain Research, 49,
159-165.
Hasselmo, M.E., Hay, J., Ilyn, M., &
Gorchetchnikov, A. (2002). Neuromodulation, theta rhythm and rat
spatial navigation. Neural Networks, 15, 689-707.
Hatfield, T., Han, J.S., Conley, M.,
Gallagher, M., & Holland, P. (1996). Neurotoxic lesions of
basolateral, but not central, amygdala interfere with Pavlovian
second-order conditioning and reinforcer devaluation effects.
Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 5256-5265.
Haviland, S.E., & Clark, H.H. (1974). What's
new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13(5),
512-521.
Hays, G.C., Åkesson, S., Broderick, A.C.,
Glen, F., Godley, B.J., Papi, F., & Luschi, P. (2003).
Island-finding ability of marine turtles. Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London, Series B, 270 (supplement),
S5-S7.
Hayward, A., McGregor, A., Good, M.A., &
Pearce, J.M. (2003). Absence of overshadowing and blocking
between landmarks and geometric cues provided by the shape of a
test arena. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
56B, 114-126.
Healy, S.D. (1998) Spatial representation
in animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Healy, S.D., Braham, S.R., & Braithwaite,
V.A. (1999). Spatial working memory in rats: No differences
between the sexes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London, Series B, 266, 2303-2308.
Healy, S.D., Clayton, N.S., & Krebs, J.R.
(1994). Development of hippocampal specialization in 2 species
of tit (Parus spp). Behavioural Brain Research,
61, 23-28.
Healy, S.D., & Hurly, T.A. (1995). Spatial memory in rufous
hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus): A field test. Animal
Learning & Behavior, 23, 63-68.
Healy, S.D., & Hurly, T.A. (1998). Rufous
hummingbirds' (Selasphorus rufus) memory for flowers:
Patterns or actual spatial locations? Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 24, 396-404.
Helbig, A.J. (1992). Ontogenetic stability of
inherited migratory directions in a nocturnal bird migrant:
Comparison between the first and second year of life.
Ecology, Ethology, Evolution, 4, 375-388.
Helbig, A.J. (1996). Genetic basis, mode of
inheritance and evolutionary changes of migratory directions in
paleaarctic warblers (Aves: Sylviidae). Journal of
Experimental Biology, 199(1), 49-55.
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994). A geometric
process for spatial representation in young children. Nature,
370, 57-59.
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1996). Modularity
and development: The case of spatial reorientation. Cognition,
61, 195-232.
Hermer-Vazquez, L., Spelke, E., & Katsnelson,
A. (1999). Source of flexibility in human cognition: Dual task
studies of space and language. Cognitive Psychology,
39, 3-36.
Hermer-Vazquez,
L., & Spelke, E.S. (1994). A geometric process for spatial
reorientation in young children. Nature, 370, 57-59.
Hermer-Vazquez,
L., & Spelke, E.S. (1996). Modularity and development: The case
of spatial reorientation. Cognition, 61, 195-232.
Hill, A.J., & Best, P.J. (1981). Effects of
deafness and blindness on the spatial correlates of hippocampal
unit activity in the rat. Experimental Neurology, 74,
204-217.
Hilton, S.C., & Krebs, J. R. (1990). Spatial
memory of four species of Parus: Performance in an
open-field analogue of a radial maze. The Quarterly Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 42B, 345-368.
Hirtle, S.C. (1995). Representational
structures for cognitive space: Trees, ordered trees and
semi-lattices. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 988,
327-340.
Hirtle, S.C., & Jonides, J. (1985). Evidence
of hierarchies in cognitive maps. Memory and Cognition, 13(3),
208-217.
Hodgson, Z. & Healy, S. D. (2005). Preference
for spatial cues in a non-storing songbird species. Animal
Cognition, 8, 211-213.
Holding, C.S. (1994). Further evidence for
the hierarchical representation of spatial information.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14(2), 137-147.
Hoffman, K. (1954). Versuche zu der im
richtungsfinden der Vögel enthaltenen zeitschätzung.
Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 11, 453 - 475.
Hollup, S.A., Molden, S., Donnett, J.G.,
Moser, M.B., & Moser, E.I. (2001). Accumulation of hippocampal
place fields at the goal location in an annular water maze task.
Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 1635-1644.
Holt, W., Maren, S. (1999). Muscimol
inactivation of the dorsal hippocampus impairs contextual
retrieval of fear memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 19,
9054-9062.
Hough II, G.E., & Bingman, V.P. (2004).
Spatial response properties of homing pigeon hippocampal
neurons: Correlations with goal locations, movement between
goals, and environmental context in a radial-arm arena. Journal
of Comparative Physiology A, 190, 1047-1062.
Houk, J.C. (1995). Information processing in
modular circuits linking basal ganglia and cerebral cortex. In
J.C Houk, J.L. Davis, & D.G. Beiser (Eds.), Models of
information processing in the basal ganglia (pp. 3-10).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hurly, T.A. & Healy, S.D. (1996). Memory for
flowers in rufous hummingbirds: Location or local visual cues?
Animal Behaviour, 51, 1149-1157.
Hurly, T.A. & Healy, S.D. (2002). Cue
learning by rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus).
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,
28, 209-223.
Hutchison, B.A., & Hicks, D.D. (Eds.).
(1985). The Forest-atmosphere interaction: Proceedings of
the Forest Environmental Measurements Conference, Oak Ridge,
Tennessee. Dordrecht, Holland: D. Reidel.
Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M. (2003). How
toddlers represent enclosed spaces. Cognitive Science,
27, 749-766.
Ikonen, S., McMahan, R., Gallagher, M.,
Eichenbaum, H., & Tanila, H. (2002). Cholinergic system
regulation of spatial representation by the hippocampus.
Hippocampus, 12, 386-397.
Ioalé, P. (1984). Magnets and pigeon
orientation. Monitore Zoologico Italiano (N.S.), 18,
347-358.
Ioalé, P., Gagliardo, A., & Bingman, V.P.
(2000). Hippocampal participation in navigational map learning
in young homing pigeons is dependent on training experience.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 742-750.
Ioalé, P., Papi, F., Fiaschi, V., & Baldaccini, N.E. (1978).
Pigeon navigation: effects upon homing behaviour by reversing
wind direction at the loft. Journal of Comparative
Physiology, 128, 285-295.
Jackendoff, R., & Landau, B. (1991). Spatial
language and spatial cognition. In D. J. Napoli & J. A. Kegl
(Eds.), Bridges between psychology and linguistics: A
swarthmore festschrift for lila gleitman (pp. 145-169).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Jacobs, L.F., & Schenk, F. (2003). Unpacking
the cognitive map: The parallel map theory of hippocampal
function. Psychological Review, 110, 285-315.
Jackson, D.E., Holcombe, M., & Ratnieks,
F.L.W. (2004). Trail geometry gives polarity to ant foraging
networks. Nature, 432, 907-909.
Jackson, R.R., & Wilcox, R.S.
(1993). Observations in nature of detouring behaviour by
Portia fimbriata, a web-invading agressive mimic jumping
spider from Queensland. Journal of Zoology, London, 230,
135-139.
Jakab, R.L., & Leranth, C. (1995). Septum. In
G. Paxinos (Ed.), The rat nervous system (2nd ed., pp.
405-442). Australia: Academic.
James, T.W., & Kimura, D. (1997). Sex
differences in remembering the locations of objects in an array:
Location-shifts versus location-exchanges. Evolution and
Human Behavior, 18, 155-163.
Jay, T.M., & Witter, M.P. (1991).
Distribution of hippocampal CA1 and subicular efferents in the
prefrontal cortex of the rat studied by means of anterograde
transport of Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin. Journal of
Comparative Neurology, 313, 574-586.
Jeffery, K.J. (Ed.) (2003). The
neurobiology of spatial behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University
Press..
Johnson, H.C. (1900). The breeding home of
the Clark's nutcracker. Condor, 2, 49-52.
Johnson, L.S., Marzluff, J.M., & Balda, R.P.
(1987). Handling of pinyon pine seeds by Clark's nutcracker.
Condor, 89, 117-125.
Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983). Mental
models: Toward a cognitive science of language, inference, and
consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Joly, P., & Miaud, C. (1989). Fidelity to the
breeding site in the alpine newt Triturus alpestris.
Behavioural Processes, 19,
47-56
Joly, P., & Miaud, C. (1993). How does
a newt finds its pond? The role of chemical cues in migrating
newts. Ethology, Ecology, Evolution, 5, 447-455.
Jones, C.M., Braithwaite, V.A., & Healy, S.D.
(2003). The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability.
Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 403-411.
Jones, J.E., & Kamil, A.C. (2001). The use of
relative and absolute bearings by Clark's nutcrackers,
Nucifraga columbiana. Animal Learning & Behavior, 29,
120-132.
Jung, M.W., Qin, Y., McNaughton, B.L.,
Barnes, C.A. (1998). Firing characteristics of deep layer
neurons in prefrontal cortex in rats performing spatial working
memory tasks. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 437-450.
Kahn, M.C., Bingman, V.P. (2004).
Lateralization of spatial learning in the avian hippocampal
formation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 333-344.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The
simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky
(Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases
(pp. 201-208). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kamil, A.C. (1988). A synthetic approach to
the study of animal intelligence. In D.W. Leger (Ed.),
Nebraska symposium on motivation: Comparative perspectives in
modern psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 230-257). Lincoln, Nebraska:
University of Nebraska Press.
Kamil, A.C., & Balda, R.P. (1990). Spatial
memory in seed caching corvids. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The
psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 26, pp. 1-25).
New York: Academic Press.
Kamil, A.C., & Cheng, K. (2001). Way-finding
and landmarks: The multiple-bearings hypothesis. Journal of
Experimental Biology, 204, 103-113.
Kamil, A.C., & Jones, J.E. (1997). Clark's
nutcrackers learn geometric relationships among landmarks.
Nature, 390, 276‑279.
Kamil, A.C. & Jones, J.E. (1997). The
seed-storing corvid Clark's nutcracker learns geometric
relationships among landmarks. Nature, 390,
276-279.
Kamil, A.C., & Jones, J.E. (2000). Geometric
rule learning by Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana).
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,
26, 439-453.
Kamil, A.C., Goodyear, A.J., & Cheng, K.
(2001). The use of landmarks by Clark's nutcrackers: First tests
of a new model. Journal of Navigation, 54, 429-435.
Kamil, A.C., Balda, R.P., & Good, S.
(1999). Patterns of movement and orientation during caching and
recovery by Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana). Animal
Behaviour, 57, 1327-1335.
Kamil, A.C., Balda, R.P., & Olson, D.J.
(1994). Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in the
radial maze analog. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108,
385-393.
Kang, E., & Gabriel, M. (1998). Hippocampal
modulation of cingulo-thalamic neuronal activity and
discriminative avoidance learning in rabbits. Hippocampus, 8,
491-510.
Kanit, L., Taskiran, D., Furedy, J. J.,
Kulali, B., McDonald, R. & Pogun, S. (1998). Nicotine interacts
with sex in affecting rat choice between "look-out" and
"navigational" cognitive styles in the Morris water maze place
learning task. Brain Research Bulletin, 46,
441-445.
Kanit, L., Taskiran, D., Yilmaz, O. A.,
Balkan, B., Demirgoren, S., Furedy, J.J., & Pogun, S. (2000).
Sexually dimorphic cognitive style in rats emerges after
puberty. Brain Research Bulletin, 52, 243-248.
Kapp, B.S., Frysinger, R.C., Gallagher, M., &
Haselton, J.R. (1979). Amygdala central nucleus lesions: Effect
on heart rate conditioning in the rabbit. Physiology and
Behavior, 23, 1109-1117.
Kavaliers, M., & Galea, L.A.M. (1995). Sex
differences in the expression and antagonism of swim
stress-induced analgesia in deer mice vary with the breeding
season. Pain, 63, 327-334.
Kavaliers, M., Ossenkopp, K.P., Prato, F.S.,
Innes, D.G.L., Galea, L.A.M., Kinsella, D.M., & Perrot-Sinal,
T.S. (1996). Spatial learning in deer mice: Sex differences and
the effects of endogenous opioids and 60 Hz magnetic fields.
Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 179, 715-724.
Keiser, J.T., Ziegenfus, C.W.S. & Cristol,
D.A. (2005). Homing success of migrant versus nonmigrant
dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis). Auk, 122,
608-617.
Keith, J.R., & McVety, K.M. (1988). Latent
place learning in a novel environment and the influences of
prior training in rats. Psychobiology, 16 (2), 146-151.
Kelly, D., & Spetch, M.L. (2001). Pigeons
encode relative geometry. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 417-422.
Kelly, D., & Spetch, M.L. (2004a).
Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment I: Do adults
encode the featural and geometric properties of a
two-dimensional schematic of a room? Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 118, 82-94.
Kelly, D., & Spetch, M.L. (2004b).
Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: II. Do pigeons (Columba
livia) encode the featural and geometric properties of a
two-dimensional schematic of a room? Journal of
Comparative Psychology, 118, 384-395.
Kelly, D., Spetch, M.L., & Heth, C.D. (1998).
Pigeon’s encoding of geometric and featural properties of a
spatial environment. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
112, 259-269.
Kendeou, P., Rapp, D.N., & van den Broek, P.
(2004). The influence of readers' prior knowledge on text
comprehension and learning from text. In R. Nata (Ed.),
Progress in education (pp. 189-209). New York: Nova Science
Publishers, Inc.
Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2005). The
effects of reader's misconceptions on comprehension of
scientific text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97,
235-245.
Kennedy, P. J., & Shapiro, M. L., (2004). Retrieving memories
via internal context requires the hippocampus. Journal of
Neuroscience, 24, 6979-6985.
Kesner, R.P., & Williams, J.M. (1995). Memory
for magnitude of reinforcement: Dissociation between the
amygdala and hippocampus. Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory, 64, 237-244.
Kiepenheuer, J. (1978). Pigeon homing: A
repetition of the deflector loft experiment. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology, 3, 393-395.
Kiepenheuer, J. (1979). Pigeon homing:
Deprivation of olfactory information does not affect the
deflector loft effect. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 6,
11-22.
Kiepenheuer, J. (1982). Pigeon orientation: A
preliminary evaluation of factors involved or not involved in
the deflector loft effect. In F. Papi &
H.G. Wallraff (Eds.), Avian navigation (pp. 203-210).
Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag.
Kiepenheuer, J. (1985). Can pigeons be
fooled about the actual release site position by presenting them
information from another site. Behavioral Ecology and
Sociobiology, 18, 75-82.
Kim, J.J., & Fanselow, M.S. (1992).
Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science, 256,
675-677.
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T.A. (1978). Toward
a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological
Review, 85(5), 363-394.
Knierim, J.J., Kudrimoti, H.S., & McNaughton,
B.L. (1998) Interactions between idiothetic cues and external
landmarks in the control of place cells and head direction
cells. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 425-446.
Kuipers, B. (1978). Modelling spatial
knowledge. Cognitive Science, 2, 129-153.
Kobayashi, T., Tran, A.H., Nishijo, H., Ono,
T., & Matsumoto, G. (2003). Contribution of hippocampal place
cell activity to learning and formation of goal-directed
navigation in rats. Neuroscience, 117, 1025-1035.
Kohonen, T. (1977). Associative memory.
Berlin, Germany: Springer Verlag.
Kramer, G. (1952). Experiments on bird
orientation. Ibis, 94, 265 - 285.
Kramer, G. (1959). Recent experiment on bird
orientation. Ibis, 101, 399 - 416.
Krebs, J.R., Healy, S.D., & Shettleworth, S.J.
(1990). Spatial memory of Paridae: Comparison of a storing and a
non-storing species, the coal tit Parus ater, and the
great tit, P.major. Animal Behaviour, 39,
1127-1137.
Krebs, J.R., Sherry, D.F., Healy, S.D.,
Perry, V.H., & Vaccarino, A.L. (1989). Hippocampal
specialization of food-storing birds. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
86, 1388-1392.
Krushinskaya, N. (1966). Some complex forms
of feeding behavior of nutcracker Nucifraga caryocatactes,
after removal of old cortex. Zhurnal Evolyutsionnoi Biokhimii
i Fiziologii, 563-568. Translated summary at:
http://soma.npa.uiuc.edu/courses/physl490b/models/bird_caching/krushinskaya_trans.html.
Kubie, J.L., & Muller, R.U. (1991). Multiple
representations in the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 1,
240-242.
Kuipers, B. (2000). The spatial semantic
hierarchy. Artificial Intelligence, 119, 191-233.
LaManna, J.R., & Eason, P.K. (2003). Effects
of landmarks on territorial establishment. Animal Behaviour,
65, 471-478.
Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). "What"
and "where" in spatial language and spatial cognition.
Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 16(2), 217-264.
Lanner, R.M. (1981).The pinyon pine: A
natural and cultural history. Reno, Nevada: University of
Nevada Press.
Latombe, J.C. (1991). Robot motion
planning, Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Lavoie, A.M., & Mizumori, S.J.Y. (1994).
Spatial, movement- and reward-sensitive discharge by medial
ventral striatum neurons of rats. Brain Research, 638,
157-168.
Learmonth, A.E., Nadel, L., & Newcombe, N.S.
(2002). Children’s use of landmarks: Implications for modularity
theory. Psychological Science, 13, 337-341.
Learmonth, A.E., Newcombe, N.S., &
Huttenlocher, J. (2001). Toddlers’ use of metric information and
landmarks to reorient. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 80, 225-244.
Lebowitz, B.K., & Brown, M.F. (1999). Sex differences in spatial
search and pattern learning in the rat. Psychobiology, 27,
364-371.
LeDoux, J.E., Cicchetti, P., Xagoraris, A., & Romanski, L.M.
(1990). The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: Sensory interface of the
amygdala in fear conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience, 10,
1062-1069.
Leiser, D., & Zilbershatz, A. (1989). The traveller: A
computational model of spatial network learning. Environment
and Behavior, 21, 435-463.
Lenck-Santini, P.P., Save, E., & Poucet, B. (2001). Evidence for
a relationship between place-cell spatial firing and spatial
memory performance. Hippocampus, 11, 377-390.
Leutgeb, S., & Mizumori, S.J.Y. (1999). Excitotoxic septal
lesions result in spatial memory deficits and altered
flexibility of hippocampal single-unit representations.
Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 6661-6672.
Leutgeb, S., Ragozzino, K.E., & Mizumori, S.J.Y. (2000).
Convergence of head direction and place information in the CA1
region of hippocampus. Neuroscience, 100, 11-19.
Levelt, W.J.M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to
articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lever, C., Wills, T., Caccucci, F., Burgess,
N., & O’Keefe, J. (2002). Long term plasticity in hippocampal
place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature,
416, 90-94.
Leyton, M. (1992). Symmetry, causality,
mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ligon, J.D. (1978). Reproductive interdependence on the pinyon
jay and pinyon pines. Ecological Monographs, 48, 111-126.
Ligon, J.D., & Martin, D.J. (1974). Pinyon seeds assessment by
the pinyon jay, Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus. Animal Behaviour,
22, 421-429.
Linde, C., & Labov, W. (1975). Spatial networks as a site for
the study of language and thought. Language, 51(4),
924-939.
Lipp, H-P, Vyssotski, A.L., Wolfer, D.P., Renaudineau, S.,
Savini, M., Tröster, G., & Dell’Omo, G. (2004). Pigeon homing
along highways and exits. Current Biology, 14, 1239-1249.
Lock, A., & Collett, T.S. (1979). A toad's
devious approach to prey: A study of some complex uses of depth
vision. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 131, 179-189.
Lohmann, K.J., & Lohmann, C.M.F. (1996).
Orientation and open-sea navigation in sea turtles. Journal
of Experimental Biology, 199, 73-81.
Lohmann, K.J., Lohmann, C.M.F., Ehrhart, L.M., Bagley, D.A., &
Swing, T. (2004). Animal behaviour: Geomagnetic map used in
sea-turtle navigation . Nature, 428, 909-910.
Lorincz, A., & Buzsaki, G. (2000). Two-phase computational model
training long-term memories in the entorhinal-hippocampal
region. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 911,
83-111.
Maaswinkel, H., Jarrard, L.E., & Whishaw, I.Q. (1999).
Hippocampectomized rats are impaired in homing by path
integration. Hippocampus, 9, 553-561.
MacEachren, A.M. (1994). Some truth with maps: A primer on
symbolization and design. Washington, D.C.: Assocation of
American Geographers.
MacEachren, A.M. (1995). How maps work: Representation,
visualization, and design. New York: Guilford Press.
MacFadden, A., Elias, L., & Saucier, D. (2003).Males and females
scan maps similarly, but give directions differently. Brain
and Cognition, 53, 297-300.
Mackintosh, J.H. (1973). Factors affecting
the recognition of territory boundaries by mice (Mus musculus).
Animal Behaviour, 21, 464-470.
Macphail, E.M., & Bolhuis, J.J. (2001). The evolution of
intelligence: Adaptive specializations versus general
process. Biological Reviews, 76, 341-364.
Macuda, T., & Roberts, W.A. (1995). Further evidence for
hierarchical chunking in rat spatial memory. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21,
20-32.
Maguire, E.A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J.G.,
Frackowiak, R.S.J., Frith, C.D., & O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing
where and getting there: A human navigation network. Science,
280, 921-924.
Maguire, E.A., Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J.
(1999). Human spatial navigation: Cognitive maps, sexual
dimorphism, and neural substrates. Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, 9, 171-177.
Maguire, E.A., Frith, C.D., Burgess, N.,
Donnett, J.G., & O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing where things are:
Parahippocampal involvement in encoding object locations in
virtual large-scale space. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
10, 61-76.
Maren, S. (2001). Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning.
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 2, 897-931.
Margules, J., & Gallistel, C.R. (1988).
Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape.
Animal Learning & Behavior, 16, 404-410.
Markus, E.J., Barnes, C.A., McNaughton, B.L., Gladden, V.L., &
Skaggs, W.E. (1994). Spatial information content and reliability
of hippocampal CA1 neurons: Effects of visual input.
Hippocampus, 4, 410-421.
Markus, E.J., Qin, Y.L., Leonard, B., Skaggs, W.E., McNaughton,
B.L., & Barnes, C.A. (1995). Interactions between location and
task affect the spatial and directional firing of hippocampal
neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 7079-7094.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Marzluff, J.M., & Balda, R.P. (1992). The Pinyon jay: Behavioral
ecology of a colonial and cooperative corvid. London, U.K.: T. &
A.D. Poyser.
Mataric, M.J. (1991). Navigating with a rat brain: A
neurobiologically-inspired model of robot spatial
representation. In From Animals to animats (Vol.
1, pp. 169-175). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Matthews, G.V.T. (1963). The orientation of pigeons as affected
by the learning of landmarks and by the distance of
displacement. Animal Behaviour, 11, 310-317.
May, M. (2004). Imaginal perspective switches in remembered
environments: Transformation versus interference accounts.
Cognitive Psychology, 48(2), 163-206.
Mayer, R.E. (2001). Multimedia learning. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Mayer, R.E. (2003). The promise of multimedia learning: Using
the same instruction design methods across different media.
Learning and Instruction, 13(2), 125-139.
Mayer, R.E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive
constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more
material results in less understanding. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 93(1), 187-198.
Mayer, R.E., & Sims, V.K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a
thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multimedia
learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3),
389-401.
McBurney, D.H., Gaulin, S.J.C., Devineni, T. & Adams, C. (1997).
Superior spatial memory of women: Stronger evidence for the
gathering hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18,
165-174.
McDonald, R.J., White, N.M. (1993). A triple dissociation of
memory systems: Hippocampus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum.
Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 3-22.
McGeorge, A.J., & Faull, R.L. (1989). The organization of the
projection from the cerebral cortex to the striatum in the rat.
Neuroscience, 29, 503-537.
McGregor, A., Good, M.A. & Pearce, J.M. (2004). Absence of an
interaction between navigational strategies based on local and
distal landmarks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 30, 34-44.
McGregor, A. & Healy, S.D. (1999). Spatial accuracy in
food-storing and nonstoring birds. Animal Behaviour,
58, 727-734.
McGregor, A., Jones, P.M.,
Good, M.A., & Pearce, J.M. (2006). Further evidence that rats
rely on local rather than global spatial information to locate a
hidden goal: Reply to Cheng and Gallistel (2005). Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32,
314-321.
McNamara, T.P., Hardy, J.K., & Hirtle, S.C. (1989). Subjective
hierarchies in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 211-227.
McNamara, T.P., Rump, B., & Werner, S. (2003). Egocentric and
geocentric frames of reference in memory of large-scale space.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 589-595.
McNamara, T. P., & Shelton, A. L. (2003). Cognitive maps and the
hippocampus. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(8), 333-335.
McNaughton, B.L., Barnes, C.A., Gerrard, J.L., Gothard, K.,
Jung, M.W., Knierim, J.J., Kudrimoti, H., Qin, Y., Skaggs, W.E.,
Suster, M., & Weaver, K.L. (1996). Deciphering the hippocampal
polyglot: The hippocampus as a path integration system.
Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 173-185.
McNaughton, B.L., Barnes, C.A., & O'Keefe, J. (1983). The
contributions of position, direction, and velocity to single
unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats.
Experimental Brain Research, 52, 41-49.
Meck, W.H., & Williams C.L. (1997). Perinatal choline
supplementation increases the threshold for chunking in spatial
memory, Neuroreport, 8(14), 3053-3059.
Mehta, M.R., Barnes, C.A., & McNaughton, B.L. (1997).
Experience-dependent, asymmetric expansion of hippocampal place
fields. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of America, 94, 8918-8921.
Merriam, C.H. (1890). Results of a biological survey of the San
Francisco Mountain region and desert of the Little Colorado,
Arizona. In North American Fauna (No.3, I-vii,
pp.1-136).U.S. Department of Agriculture.
Miller, G.A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus
two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.
Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.
Miller, G.A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K.H. (1960). Plans and
the structure of behavior. New York: Holt.
Miller, G.A., & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1976). Language and
perception. Oxford, England: Harvard University Press.
Miller, R.S., Tamm, S., Sutherland, G.D., & Gass, C.L. (1985).
Cues for orientation in hummingbird foraging: Color and
position. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 63, 18-21.
Miller, V.M., & Best, P.J. (1980). Spatial correlates of
hippocampal unit activity are altered by lesions of the fornix
and entorhinal cortex. Brain Research, 194, 311-323.
Mittelstaedt, H. (2000). Triple-loop model
of path control by head direction and place cells. Biological
Cybernetics, 83, 261-270.
Mittelstaedt, M.K., & Mittelstaedt, H. (1982). Homing by path
integration. In F. Papi & H.G. Wallraff (Eds.), Avian
navigation (pp. 290-297). Berlin, Germany: Springer Verlag.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., Canfield, J.G., & Yeshenko, O. (2005).
Parallel and inter-related neural systems underlying adaptive
navigation. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 45,
547-554.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., Cooper, B.G., Leutgeb, S., & Pratt, W.E.
(2000a). A neural systems analysis of adaptive navigation.
Molecular Neurobiology, 21, 57-82.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., McNaughton, B.L., & Barnes, C.A. (1989a). A
comparison of supramammillary and medial septal influences on
hippocampal field potentials and single-unit activity.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 61, 15-31.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., McNaughton, B.L., Barnes, C.A., & Fox, K.B.
(1989b). Preserved spatial coding in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal
cells during reversible suppression of CA3c output: Evidence for
pattern completion in hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience,
9, 3915-3928.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., Pratt, W.E., Cooper, B.G., & Guazzelli, A.
(2002). The behavioral implementation of hippocampal processing.
In P.E. Sharp (Ed.), The neural basis of navigation: Evidence
from single cell recording (pp. 197-216). Boston, MA: Kluwer
Academic Publishers.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., Pratt, W.E., & Ragozzino, K.E.
(1999a). Function of the nucleus accumbens within the
context of the larger striatal system. Psychobiology, 27,
214-224
Mizumori, S.J.Y., Puryear, C.M., Gill, K.M., & Guazzelli, A.
(2004a). Head direction codes in hippocampal afferent and
efferent systems: What functions do they serve? In S.L. Wiener &
J.S. Taube (Eds.), Head direction cells and the neural
mechanisms underlying directional orientation. Boston, MA:
MIT Press.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., Ragozzino, K.E., & Cooper, B.G. (2000b).
Location and head direction representation in the dorsal
striatum of rats. Psychobiology, 28, 441-462.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., Ragozzino, K.E., Cooper, B.G., & Leutgeb, S.
(1999b). Hippocampal representational organization and spatial
context. Hippocampus, 9, 444-451.
Mizumori, S.J.Y, Ward, K.E., & Lavoie, A.M. (1992). Medial
septal modulation of entorhinal single unit activity in
anesthetized and freely moving rats. Brain Research, 570,
188-197.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., Williams, J.D. (1993). Directionally selective
mnemonic properties of neurons in the lateral dorsal nucleus of
the thalamus of rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 13,
4015-4028.
Mizumori, S.J.Y., Yeshenko, O., Gill, K.M., & Davis, D. (2004b).
Parallel processing across neural systems: Implications for a
multiple memory systems hypothesis. Neurobiology of Learning
and Memory, 82,
278-298
Moffat, S.D., Hampson, E. & Hatzipantelis, M. (1998). Navigation
in a "virtual" maze: Sex differences and correlation with
psychometric measures of spatial ability in humans. Evolution
and Human Behavior, 19, 73-87.
Montello, D.R., Lovelace, K., Golledge, R.G., & Self, C. (1999).
Sex-related differences and similarities in geographic and
environmental spatial abilities. Annals of the Association of
American Geographers, 89, 515-534.
de los Monteros, A.E., & Cracraft, L. (1997). Intergeneric
relationships of the New World jays inferred from cytochrome
b gene sequences. Condor, 99, 490-502.
Moore, B.R. (1980). Is the homing pigeon's mag geomagnetic?
Nature, 285, 69-70.
Moore, F.R. (1987). Sunset and the orientation behavior of
migratory birds. Biological Reviews, 62, 65-86.
Mora, C.V., Davidson, M., Wild, J.M., & Walker, M.M.
(2004). Magnetoreception and its trigeminal mediation in the
homing pigeon. Nature, 432,508-511.
Morris, R.G.M. (1981). Spatial localisation
does not require the presence of local cues. Learning and
Motivation, 12, 239-260.
Morrow, D.G. (1994). Spatial models created from text. In H. van
Oostendorp & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Naturalistic text
comprehension (pp. 57-78). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing
Corporation.
Morrow, D.G., Bower, G.H., & Greenspan, S.L. (1989). Updating
situation models during narrative comprehension. Journal of
Memory and Language, 28, 292-312.
Morrow, D.G., Greenspan, S.L., & Bower, G.H. (1987).
Accessibility and situation model narrative comprehension.
Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 165-187.
Mou, W., & McNamara, T.P. (2002). Intrinsic frames of reference
in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 162-170.
Mouritsen, H. (2003). Spatiotemporal orientation strategies of
long-distance migrants. In P. Berthold, E.
Gwinner, & E. Sonnenschein (Eds.), Avian Migration (pp.
493-513). Berlin: Springer Verlag.
Mouritsen, H., & Frost, B.J. (2002). Virtual migration in
tethered flying monarch butterflies reveals their orientation
mechanisms. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
of the United States of America, 99, 10162-10166.
Muheim, R., Bäckman, J., & Åkesson, S. (2002). Magnetic compass
orientation in European robins is dependent on both wavelength
and intensity of light. Journal of Experimental Biology, 205(24),
3845-3856.
Muheim, R., Moore, F.R., & Phillips, J.B. (2006). Calibration of
magnetic and celestial compass cues in migratory birds- a review of
cue-conflict experiments. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209,
2-17.
Muheim, R., Phillips, J.B., & Akesson, S. (2006). Polarized light
cues underlie compass calibration in migratory songbird. Science,
313, 837-839.
Müller, M., & Wehner, R. (1988). Path
integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 85, 5287-5290.
Muller, R.U., & Kubie, J.L. (1987). The effects of changes in
the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal
complex-spike cells. Journal of Neuroscience, 7,
1951-1968.
Muller, R.U., Kubie, J.L., & Saypoff, R. (1991). The hippocampus
as a cognitive graph (abridged version). Hippocampus, 1,
243-246.
Muller, R.U., & Stead, M. (1996). Hippocampal place cells
connected by Hebbian synapses can solve spatial problems.
Hippocampus, 6, 709-719.
Muller, R.U., Stead. M., & Pach, J. (1996).
The hippocampus as a cognitive graph. Journal of General
Physiology, 107, 663-694.
Munro, U., Munro, J.A., & Phillips, J.B. (1997). Evidence for a
magnetite-based navigational 'map' in birds.
Naturwissenschaften, 84, 26-28.
Munro, U., Munro, J.A., Phillips, J.B., & Wiltschko, W. (1997).
Effect of wavelength of light and pulse magnetization on
different magnetoreception systems in a migratory bird.
Australian Journal of Zoology, 45, 189-198.
Myers, C.E., & Gluck, M. (1994). Context, conditioning, and
hippocampal representation in animal learning. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 108, 835-847.
Nadel, L., Willner, J., & Kurz, E.M. (1985). Cognitive maps and
environmental context. In P. Balsam & A. Tomie (Eds.),
Context and Learning (pp. 385-406). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nehmzow, U. (1995). Animal and robot navigation. Robotics and
Autonomous Systems, 15, 71-81.
Nevitt, G.A., Reid, K., & Trathan, P. (2004). Testing olfactory
foraging strategies in an Antarctic seabird assemblage.
Journal of Experimental Biology, 207,
3537-3544.
Newcombe, N.S. (2002). The
nativist-empiricist controversy in the context of recent
research on spatial and quantitative development.
Psychological Science, 13, 395-401.
Newcombe, N.S., & Sluzenski, J. (2004). Knowing where things are
in the second year of life: Implications for hippocampal
development. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. Special
Issue on Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8),
1443-1451.
Niizuma, M., Kawano, Y., Tomizawa, M., Sugiyama, M., & Degawa,
S. (1992). Flexible control of autonomous vehicle utilizing
hierarchical map and planning, Proceedings of the IMACS/SICE
International Symposium on Robotics, Mechatronics and
Manufacturing Systems, Japan.
Nolfi, S. (2002). Power and limits of
reactive agents. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 42,
119-145.
Odling-Smee, L., & Braithwaite, V. A. (2003). The influence of
habitat stability on landmark use during spatial learning in the
three-spined stickleback. Animal Behaviour, 65,
701-707.
O‘Keefe, J. (1976). Place units in the
hippocampus of the freely moving rat. Experimental
Neurology, 51, 78-109.
O’Keefe, J. (1996). The spatial prepositions in English, vector
grammar, and the cognitive map theory. In L. Bloom, M.A.
Peterson, L. Nadel, & M.F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space
(pp. 277-316). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
O'Keefe, J., & Burgess, N. (1996). Geometric
determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons.
Nature, 381, 425-428.
O’Keefe, J., Burgess, N., Donnett, J.G.,
Jeffery, K.J., & Maguire, E.A. (1999). Place cells, navigational
accuracy, and the human hippocampus. In G.W. Humphreys, J.
Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space, and action:
Studies in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 153-164). Oxford,
U.K.: Oxford University Press.
O'Keefe, J., & Conway, D.H. (1978). Hippocampal place units in
the freely moving rat: Why they fire where they fire.
Experimental Brain Research, 31, 573-590.
O'Keefe, J., & Dostrovsky, J. (1971). The hippocampus as a
spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the
freely moving rat. Brain Research, 34, 171-175.
O‘Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The
hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Olson, D.J. (1991). Species differences in spatial memory among
Clark's nutcrackers, scrub jays, and pigeons. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17,
363-376.
Olson, D.J., Kamil, A.C., Balda, R.P., & Nims, P.J. (1995).
Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in operant tests
of nonspatial and spatial memory. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 109, 173-181.
Olthof, A., Sutton, J.E., Slumskie, S. V., D'Addetta, J., &
Roberts, W.A. (1999). In search of the cognitive map: Can rats
learn an abstract pattern of rewarded arms on the radial maze?
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior
Processes, 25, 352-362.
Olton, D.S., Branch, M., & Best, P.J. (1978). Spatial correlates
of hippocampal unit activity. Experimental Neurology, 58,
387-409.
Olton, D.S., & Samuelson, R.J. (1976). Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2,
97-116.
O'Mara, S.M. (1995). Spatially selective firing properties of
hippocampal formation neurons in rodents and primates.
Progress in Neurobiology, 45, 253-274.
Ormerod, B.K., & Galea, L.A.M. (2003). Reproductive status
influences the survival of new cells in the dentate gyrus of
adult male meadow voles. Neuroscience Letters, 346,
25-28.
Osorio, D., Smith, A.C., Vorobyev, M., & Buchanan-Smith, H.M.
(2004). Detection of fruit and the selection of primate visual
pigments for color vision. American Naturalist, 164,
696-708.
Paine, R. T. (1963). Food recognition and
predation on Opisthobranchs by Navanax inermis. The Veliger, 6, 1-9.
Papi, F., Fiore, L., Fiaschi, V., Benvenuti, S., & Baldaccini,
N.E. (1972). Olfaction and homing in pigeons. Monitore
Zoologico Italiano (N.S.), 6, 85-95.
Papi, F. (1990). Olfactory navigation in birds.
Experientia, 46, 352-362.
Pearce, J.M., Good, M.A., Jones, P.M., &
McGregor, A. (2004). Transfer of spatial behaviour between
different environments: Implications for theories of spatial
learning and for the role of the hippocampus in spatial
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 30(2), 135-147.
Pearce, J.M., Graham, M.,
Good, M.A., Jones, P.M., & McGregor, A. (2006). Potentiation,
overshadowing, and blocking of spatial learning based on the
shape of the environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 201-214.
Pearce, J.M., Ward-Robinson, J., Good, M., Fussell, C., & Aydin,
A. (2001). Influence of a beacon on spatial learning based on
the shape of the test environment. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 329-344.
Penick, S., & Solomon, P.R. (1991). Hippocampus, context, and
conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 611-617.
Perdeck, A.C. (1958). Two types of orientation in migrating
Starlings Sturnus vulgaris and Chaffinches Fringilla
coelebs as revealed by displacement experiments. Ardea,
46, 1-37.
de Perera, T.B. (2004). Fish can encode order
in their spatial map. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London, Series B, 271, 2131-2134.
Perrot-Sinal, T.S., Kostenuik, M.A., Ossenkopp, K.-P., &
Kavaliers, M. (1996). Sex differences in performance in the
Morris water maze and the effects of initial nonstationary
hidden platform training. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110,
1309-1320.
Peterson, A.T. (1993). Adaptive geographical variation in bill
shape of scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens).
American Naturalist, 142, 508-527.
Phillips, J.B. (1986a). Magnetic compass orientation in the
Eastern red-spotted newt Notophthalmus viridescens. Journal
of Comparative Physiology A, 158, 103-109.
Phillips, J.B. (1986b). Two magnetoreceptor pathways in a
migratory salamander. Science, 233, 765-767.
Phillips, J.B. (1996). Magnetic navigation. Journal of
Theoretical Biology, 180, 309-319.
Phillips, J.B., Adler, K., & Borland, S.C. (1995). True
navigation by an amphibian. Animal Behaviour, 50,
855-858.
Phillips, J.B., & Borland, S.C. (1992a). Magnetic compass
orientation is eliminated under near-infrared light in the
Eastern red-spotted newt, Notophthalmus viridescens.
Animal Behaviour, 44, 796-797.
Phillips, J.B., & Borland, S.C. (1992b). Wavelength-specific
effects of light on magnetic compass orientation of the Eastern
red-spotted newt Notophthalmus viridescens. Ecology,
Ethology, Evolution, 4, 33-42.
Phillips, J.B., & Borland, S.C. (1994). Use
of a specialized magnetoreception system for homing by the
Eastern red-spotted newt, Notophthalmus viridescens.
Journal of Experimental Biology, 188, 275-291.
Phillips, J.B., Borland, S.C., Freake, M.J., Brassart, J., &
Kirschvink, J.L. (2002a). `Fixed-axis' magnetic orientation by
an amphibian: Non-shoreward-directed compass orientation,
misdirected homing or positioning a magnetite-based map detector
in a consistent alignment relative to the magnetic field?
Journal of Experimental Biology, 205(24), 3903-3914.
Phillips, J.B., & Deutschlander, M.E. (1997). Magnetoreception
in terrestrial vertebrates: Implications for possible mechanisms
of EMF interaction with biological systems. In R.G. Stevens,
L.E. Andrews, & B.W. Wilson (Eds.), The melatonin
hypothesis: Electric power and the risk of breast cancer
(pp. 111-172). Columbus, OH: Battelle Press.
Phillips, J.B., Freake, M.J., Fischer, J.H., & Borland, S.C.
(2002b). Behavioural titration of a magnetic map coordinate.
Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 188, 157-160.
Phillips, J.B., & F.R. Moore (1992).
Calibration of the sun‑compass of a migratory bird by sunset
polarized light patterns. Behavioral Ecology &
Sociobiology, 31,189‑194.
Phillips, J.B., & Waldvogel, J.A. (1982). Reflected light cues
generate the short-term deflector-loft effect. In F. Papi & E.
Wallraff (Eds.), Avian navigation (pp.
190-202). Berlin, Springer-Verlag.
Phillips, J.B., & Waldvogel, J.A. (1988). Celestial polarized
patterns as a calibration reference for sun compass of homing
pigeons. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 131, 55-67.
Phillips, R.G., & LeDoux, J.E. (1994). Lesions of the dorsal
hippocampal formation interfere with background but not
foreground contextual fear conditioning. Learning & Memory, 1,
34-44.
Pitelka, F.A. (1951). Speciation and ecologic distribution of
American jays of the genus Aphelocoma. University of
California Publications in Zoology, 50, 195-463.
Pizer, S.M., Fritsch, D.S., Yushkevich, P.A.,
Johnson, V.E., & Chaney, E.L. (1999). Segmentation,
registration, and measurement of shape variation via image
object shape. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 18,
851-865.
Ploeger, G.E., Spruijt, B.M., & Cools, A.R. (1994). Spatial
localization in the Morris water maze in rats: Acquisition is
affected by intra-accumbens injections of the dopaminergic
antagonist haloperidol. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108,
927-934.
Plumert, J. M., Ewert, K., & Spear, S.J. (1995). The early
development of children's communication about nested spatial
relations. Child Development, 66, 959-969.
Postma, A., Izendoorn, R., & De Haan, E.H.F. (1998). Sex
differences in object location memory. Brain and Cognition,
36, 334-345.
Poucet, B. (1997). Searching for spatial unit firing in the
prelimbic area of the rat medial prefrontal cortex.
Behavioural Brain Research, 84, 151-159.
Poucet, B., & Herrmann, T. (1990). Septum and medial frontal
cortex contribution to spatial problem-solving. Behavioural
Brain Research, 37, 269-280.
Poucet, B., Lenck-Santini, P.P., & Save, E.
(2003). Drawing parallels between the behavioural and neural
properties of navigation. In K.J. Jeffery (Ed.), The
neurobiology of spatial behaviour (pp. 187-198). Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Poucet, B., Save, E., & Lenck-Santini, P.P. (2000). Sensory and
memory properties of hippocampal place cells. Reviews in the
Neurosciences, 11, 95-111.
Pratt, W.E., & Mizumori, S.J.Y. (1998). Characteristics of
basolateral amygdala neuronal firing on a spatial memory task
involving differential reward. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112,
554-570.
Pratt, W.E., & Mizumori, S.J.Y. (2001). Neurons in rat medial
prefrontal cortex show anticipatory rate changes to predictable
differential rewards in a spatial memory task. Behavioural
Brain Research, 123, 165-183.
Presson, C. C., & Montello, D. R. (1994). Updating after
rotational and translational body movement: Coordinate
structure of perspective space. Perception, 23(12),
1447-1455.
Quirk, G.J., Muller, R.U., & Kubie, J.L. (1990). The firing of
hippocampal place cells in the dark depends on the rat's recent
experience. Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 2008-2017.
Quirk, G.J., Muller, R.U., Kubie, J.L., & Ranck, J.B., Jr.
(1992). The positional firing properties of medial entorhinal
neurons: Description and comparison with hippocampal place
cells. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 1945-1963.
Ragozzino, M.E., Pal, S.N., Unick, K., Stefani, M.R., & Gold,
P.E. (1998). Modulation of hippocampal acetylcholine release and
spontaneous alternation scores by intrahippocampal glucose
injections. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 1595-1601.
Ranck, J.B. (1973). Studies on single neurons in dorsal
hippocampal formation and septum in unrestrained rats. I.
Behavioral correlates and firing repertoires. Experimental
Neurology, 41, 461-531.
Rapp, D.N. (2005). Mental models: Theoretical issues for
visualizations in science education. In J. K. Gilbert (Ed.),
Visualization in science education (pp. 43-60). The
Netherlands: Springer.
Rapp, D.N., Klug, J.L., & Taylor, H.A. (in press).
Expectation-driven effects on spatial mental model use during
reading. Memory & Cognition.
Rapp, D.N., & Taylor, H.A. (2004). Interactive dimensions in the
construction of mental representastions for text. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30,
988-1001.
Rapp, D.N., Taylor, H.A., & Crane, G. (2003). The impact of
digital libraries on cognitive processes: Psychological issues
of hypermedia. Computers in Human Behavior, 19, 609-628.
Rapp, D.N., & van den Broek, P. (2005). Dynamic text
comprehension: An integrative view of reading. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 276-279.
Redish, A.D. (2001). The hippocampal debate: Are we asking the
right questions? Behavioural Brain Research, 127, 81-98.
Reep, R.L., & Corwin, J.V. (1999). Topographic organization of
the striatal and thalamic connections of rat medical agranular
cortex. Brain Research, 84, 43-52.
Reep, R.L., Corwin, J.V., & Hashimoto, A. (1997). Efferent
connections of the rostral portion of medial agranular cortex in
rats. Brain Research Bulletin, 19, 203-221.
Reep, R.L., Corwin, J.V., Hashimoto, A., & Watson, R.T. (1984).
Afferent connections of medial precentral cortex in the rat.
Neuroscience Letters, 44, 247-252.
Reep, R.L., Goodwin, G.S., & Corwin, J.V. (1990). Topographic
organization in the corticocortical connections of medial
agranular cortex in rats. Journal of Comparative Neurology,
294, 262-280.
Rehkamper, G., Haase, E., & Frahm, H.D. (1988). Allometric
comparison of brain weight and brain structure volumes in
different breeds of the domestic pigeon. Brain, Behaviour and
Evolution, 31, 141-149.
Reid, A.K., & Staddon, J.E.R. (1998). A dynamic route finder for
the cognitive map. Psychological Review, 105, 585-601.
Reitman, J.S., & Rueter, HR. (1980). Organization revealed by
recall orders and confirmed by pauses. Cognitive Psychology,
12, 554-581.
Rescorla, R.A. (1991). Associative relations
in instrumental learning: The eighteenth Bartlett memorial
lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B,
1-23.
Rich, S.S., & Taylor, H.A. (2000). Not all narrative shifts
function equally. Memory & Cognition, 28, 1257-1266.
Rieser, J.J. (1989). Access to knowledge of spatial structure at
novel points of observation. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6),
1157-1165.
Rieser, J.J., Garing, A.E., & Young, M.F. (1994). Imagery,
action, and young children's spatial orientation: It's not
being there that counts, it's what one has in mind. Child
Development, 65(5), 1262-1278.
Riley, D.A.,
Brown, M.F., & Yoerg, S.I.
(1986). Understanding animal cognition. In T.J. Knapp & L.C.
Robertson (Eds.) Approaches to cognition: Contrasts and
controversies (pp. 111-136). Hillsdale, N.J.:
Erlbaum.
Rinck, M., & Bower, G.H. (1995). Anaphora resolution and the
focus of attention in situation models. Journal of Memory and
Language, 34, 110-131.
Rinck, M., Bower, G.H., & Wolf, K. (1998). Distance effects in
surface structures and situation models. Scientific Studies
of Reading, 2, 221-246.
Rinck, M., Hahnel, A., Bower, G.H., & Glowalla, U. (1997). The
metrics of spatial situation models. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 622-637.
Rinck, M., Williams, P., Bower, G.H., & Becker, E.S. (1996).
Spatial situation models and narrative understanding: Some
generalizations and extensions. Discourse Processes, 21,
23-55.
Risold, P.Y., & Swanson, L.W. (1995). Evidence for a
hypothalamothalamocortical circuit mediating pheromonal
influences on eye and head movements. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 92,
3898-3902.
Ritz, T., Adem, S., & Schulten, K. (2000). A model for
photoreceptor-based magnetoreception in birds. Biophysical
Journal, 78, 707-718.
Ritz, T., Thalau, P., Phillips, J.B., Wiltschko, R., & Wiltschko,
W. (2004). Resonance effects indicate a radical-pair mechanism
for avian magnetic compass. Nature, 429(6988),
177-180.
Roberts, W.A. (1979). Spatial memory in the rat on a
hierarchical maze. Learning and Motivation, 10, 117-140.
Rodda, G.H. (1984). The orientation and
navigation of juvenile alligators: Evidence of magnetic
sensitivity. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 154,
649-658.
Rodda, G.H., & Phillips, J.B. (1992). Navigational systems
develop along similar lines in amphibians, reptiles and birds.
Ecology, Ethology, Evolution, 4, 43-51.
Romonchuk, W.J. (1995). The role of memory in cache-recovery in
Clark's nutcrackers and pinyon jays. Unpublished PhD
dissertation, Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ.
Rotenberg, A., & Muller, R.U. (1997).
Variable place-cell coupling to a
continuously viewed stimulus: Evidence that the hippocampus acts
as a perceptual system. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society of London B, 352, 1505-1513.
Saint Paul, Uv. (1953). Nachweis der
sonnenorientierung bei nächtlich ziehenden Vögeln.
Behaviour, 6, 1-7.
Samet, H. (1980). Region representation: Quadtrees from boundary
codes. Communications of the ACM, 23(2), 163-170.
Samsonovich, A., & McNaughton, B. L.
(1997). Path integration and cognitive mapping in a continuous
attractor neuronal network model. Journal of Neuroscience,
17, 5900-5920.
Sandstrom, N.J., Kaufman, J., & Huettel, S.A. (1998). Males and
females use different distal cues in a virtual environment
navigation task. Cognitive Brain Research, 6,
351-360.
Sauer, E.G.F., & Sauer, E.M. (1960). Star navigation of
nocturnal migrating birds. Cold Spring Harbor Symposium
Quantitative Biology, 25, 463-473.
Save, E., Cressant, A., Thinus-Blanc, C., & Poucet, B. (1998).
Spatial firing of hippocampal place cells in blind rats.
Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 1818-1826.
Save, E., Guazzelli, A., & Poucet, B. (2001). Dissociation of
the effects of bilateral lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and
parietal cortex on path integration in the rat. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 115, 1212-1223.
Schmajuk, N.A. (1990). Role of the hippocampus in temporal and
spatial navigation: An adaptive neural network. Behavioural
Brain Research, 39(3), 205-229.
Schmajuk, N.A., & Thieme A.D. (1992). Purposive behavior and
cognitive mapping: An adaptive neural network. Biological
Cybernetics, 67, 165-174.
Schmidt-Koenig, K. (1961). Die sonne als kompass im
heim-orientierungssystem der brieftauben. Zeitschrift für
Tierpsychologie, 68, 221 - 224.
Schmidt-Koenig, K (1966). Ueber die entfernung
als parameter bei der anfangsorientierung der brieftaube.
Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie, 52, 33-55.
Schmidt-Koenig, K. (1970). Entfernung und
heimkehrverhalten der brieftaube. Zeitschrift für
Vergleichende Physiologie, 68, 39-48.
Schmidt-Koenig, K. (1979). Avian
orientation and navigation. London, Academic Press.
Schmidt-Koenig,
K. (1990). The sun compass. Experientia, 46,
336-342.
Schmidt-Koenig, K., & Walcott, C. (1978). Tracks of pigeons
homing with frosted lenses. Animal Behaviour, 26,
480-486.
Schölkopf, B., & Mallot, H.A. (1994).
View-based cognitive mapping and path planning. Adaptive
Behaviour, 3(3), 311-348.
Schopmeyer, C.S. (1974). Seeds of wood plants in the United
States. In Agriculture, Handbook (No. 540, pp. 196-197).
Washington D.C.: U.S.D.A. Forest Service.
Schultz, A.C., Adams, W., & Yamauchi B. (1999). Integrating
exploration localization and planning with a common
representation. Autonomous Robots, 6, 293-308.
Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward.
Neuron, 36, 241-263.
Schultz, W., Apicella, P., Romo, R., & Scarnati, E. (1995).
Context-dependent activity in primate striatum reflecting past
and future behavioral events. In J.C. Houk, J.L. Davis, & D.G.
Beiser (Eds.), Models of information processing in the basal
ganglia (pp. 11-27). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P.R. (1997). A neural
substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275,
1593-1599.
Schultz, W., Tremblay, L., & Hollerman, J.R. (2003). Changes in
behavior-related neuronal activity in the striatum during
learning. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 321-328.
Scoville, W.B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after
bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurochemistry, 20,
11-21.
Seamans, J.K., Floresco, S.B., & Phillips, A.G. (1995).
Functional differences between the prelimbic and anterior
cingulate regions of the rat prefrontal cortex. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 109, 1063-1073.
Segal, M. (1979). A potent inhibitory monosynaptic
hypothalamo-hippocampal connection. Brain Research,
162, 137-141.
Séguinot, V., Maurer, R., & Etienne, A.S.
(1993). Dead reckoning in a small mammal: The evaluation of
distance. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 173,
103-113.
Sesack, S.R., Deutch, A.Y., Roth, R.H., & Bunney, B.S. (1989).
Topographical organization of the efferent projections of the
medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: An anterograde
tract-tracing study with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin.
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 290, 213-242.
Shapiro, M.L., & Eichenbaum, H. (1999). Hippocampus as a memory
map: Synaptic plasticity and memory encoding by hippocampal
neurons. Hippocampus, 9, 365-384.
Sharp, P.E. (1991). Computer-simulation of hippocampal place
cells. Psychobiology, 19(2), 103-115.
Sharp, P.E. (2002). The neural basis of navigation: Evidence
from single cell recording. Boston: Kluwer Academic
Publishers.
Sharp, P.E., Blair, H.T., & Cho, J. (2001). The anatomical and
computational basis of the rat head-direction cell signal.
Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 289-294.
Sharp, P.E., & Green, C. (1994). Spatial correlates of firing
patterns of single cells in the subiculum of the freely moving
rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 2339-2356.
Shelton, A. ., & McNamara, T.P. (1997). Multiple views of
spatial memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(1),
102-106.
Shelton, A.L., & McNamara, T.P. (2001a). Systems of spatial
refererence in human memory. Cognitive Psychology, 43(4),
274-310.
Shelton, A.L., & McNamara, T.P. (2001b). Visual memories from
nonvisual experiences. Psychological Science, 12(4),
343-347.
Sherry, D. F., & Vaccarino, A. L. (1989). Hippocampus and memory
for food caches in black-capped chickadees. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 103, 308-318.
Sherry, D.F., Vaccarino, A.L., Buckenham, K., & Herz, R.S.
(1989). The hippocampal complex of food-storing birds. Brain,
Behavior, and Evolution, 34, 308-317.
Shettleworth, S.J., Krebs, J.R., Healy, S.D., & Thomas, C.M.
(1990). Spatial memory of food-storing tits (Parus ater
and P. atricapillus): Comparison of storing and
nonstoring tasks. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
104, 71-81.
Shimizu, T., Bowers, A.N., Budzynski, C., Kahn, M.C., & Bingman,
V.P. (2004). What does a pigeon brain look like during homing?
Selective examination of ZENK expression in the telencephalon of
pigeons navigating home. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118,
845-851.
Sholl, M. J. (1987). Cognitive maps as orienting schemata.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 13, 615-628.
Sholl, M.J. (2000). The functional separability of
self-reference and object-to-object systems in spatial memory.
In S. O'Nuallain (Ed.), Spatial cognition: Foundations and
applications (pp. 45-67). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Sholl, M.J., & Nolin, T.L. (1997). Orientation specificity in
representation of place. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(6), 1494-1507.
Shulzitski, J. (1999). Sex differences in the spatial memory
ability of mated pinyon jays. Unpublished MS thesis, Northern
Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ.
Siegel, J.J., Nitz, D., & Bingman, V.P. (2005).
Spatial-specificity of single-units in the hippocampal formation
of freely moving homing pigeons. Hippocampus, 15, 26-40.
Siegel, J.J., Nitz, D., & Bingman, V.P. (2006). Lateralized
functional components of spatial cognition in the avian
hippocampal formation: Evidence from single-unit recordings in
freely moving homing pigeons. Hippocampus, 16, 125-140.
Silverman, I., Choi, J., Mackewn, A., Fisher, M., Moro, J., &
Olshansky, E. (2000). Evolved mechanisms underlying wayfinding:
Further studies on the hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex
differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21,
201-213.
Sinsch, U. (1990a). Migration and
orientation in anuran amphibians. Etholology Ecology &
Evolution, 2, 65-79.
Sinsch, U. (1990b). The orientation
behaviour of three toad species (genus Bufo) displaced from the
breeding site. In W. Hanke (Ed.) Biology and physiology
of amphibians (Vol. 38, pp. 73-83). G. Fischer Verlag,
Fortschritte der Zoologie.
Skaggs, W.E., Knierim, J.J., Kudrimoti, H.S., & McNaughton, B.L.
(1995). A model of the neural basis of the rat’s sense of
direction. In G. Tesauro, D. Touretzky, & T. Leen (Eds.)
Advances in neural information processing systems (Vol.
7). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Skaggs, W.E., & McNaughton, B.L. (1998). Spatial firing
properties of hippocampal CA1 populations in an environment
containing two visually identical regions. Journal of
Neuroscience, 18, 8455-8466.
Skiles, D.D. (1985). The geomagnetic field: Its nature, history,
and biological relevance. In J.L. Kirschvink, D.S. Jones, & B.J.
McFadden (Eds.), Magnetite biomineralization and
magnetoreception in organisms: A new biomagnetism (pp.
43-102). New York and London: Plenum Press.
Smith, D. M., & Mizumori, S. J. Y. (2006). Learning-related
development of context-specific neuronal responses to places and
events: The hippocampal role in context processing. Journal
of Neuroscience, 26, 3154-3163.
Smulders, T.V., & DeVoogd, T.J. (2000). Expression of immediate
early genes in the hippocampal formation of the black-capped
chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) during a food-hoarding
task. Behavioural Brain Research, 114, 39-49.
Sovrano, V.A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2002).
Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: Encoding
of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial
environment by fish. Cognition, 85, B51-B59.
Sovrano, V.A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2003).
Modularity as a fish (Xenotoca eiseni) views it:
Conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial
reorientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 29, 199-210.
Spencer, C., & Weetman, M. (1981). The microgenesis of cognitive
maps - A longitudinal-study of new residents of an urban area.
Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers,
6, 375-384.
Spetch, M.L. (1995). Overshadowing in landmark learning:
Touch-screen studies with pigeons and humans. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21,
166-181.
Spetch, M.L., Cheng, K., & Macdonald, S.E.
(1996). Learning the configuration of a landmark array I:
Touch-screen studies with pigeons and humans. Journal of
Comparative Psychology, 110(1), 55-68.
Spetch, M.L., Cheng, K., Macdonald, S.E.,
Linkenhoker, B. E., Kelly, D. M., & Doerkson, S. R.
(1997). Learning the configuration of a landmark array: II.
Generality across search tasks. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 111(1), 14-24.
Squire, L.R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The medial temporal lobe
memory system. Science, 253, 1380-1386.
Staddon, J.E.R. (2001). Adaptive dynamics: The theoretical
analysis of behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Stegmann, B.K. (1934). On the ontogeny of the nutcracker
(Kedrovka) (trans. L. Kelso). Dolk. Akad. Nauk. SSSR,
24, 267-269.
Steinhage, A., & Schöner, G.
(1996). Self-calibration based on invariant view recognition:
Dynamic approach to navigation. Robotics and Autonomous
Systems, 20, 133-156.
Stern, E.A., Jaeger, D., & Wilson, C.J. (1998). Membrane
potential synchrony of simultaneously recorded striatal spiny
neurons in vivo. Nature, 394, 475-478.
Stevens, A., & Coupe, P. (1978). Distortions in judged spatial
relations, Cognitive Psychology, 10(4), 422-437.
Stevens, P.S. (1974). Patterns in nature. New York:
Little, Brown & Co.
Stotz, N.G., & Balda, R. P. (1995). Cache and recovery behavior
of wild pinyon jays in Northern Arizona. Southwest
Naturalist, 40, 180-184.
Sutherland, R.J., Chew, G. L., Baker, J.C.,
& Linggard, R.C. (1987). Some limitations on the use of distal
cues in place navigation by rats. Psychobiology, 15 (1),
48-57.
Suzuki, S., Augerinos, G., & Black, A.H. (1980). Stimulus
control of spatial behavior on the eight-arm radial maze.
Learning and Motivation, 11, 1-18.
Swanson, L.W., & Cowan, W.M. (1979). The connections of the
septal region in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology,
186, 621-656.
Swanson, L.W., & Kohler, C. (1986). Anatomical evidence for
direct projections from the entorhinal area to the entire
cortical mantle in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 6,
3010-3023.
Tarsitano, M.S., & Jackson, R.R. (1997). Araneophagic
jumping spiders discriminate between detours that do and do not
lead to prey. Animal Behaviour, 53(2), 257-266.
Taube, J.S., Muller, R.U. (1998). Comparisons of head direction
cell activity in the postsubiculum and anterior thalamus of
freely moving rats. Hippocampus, 8, 87-108.
Taylor, H.A., Naylor, S. J., & Chechile, N.A. (1999).
Goal-directed influences on the representation of spatial
perspective. Memory and Cognition, 27(2), 309-319.
Taylor, H.A., & Tversky, B. (1992a). Descriptions and depictions
of environments. Memory & Cognition, 20, 483-496.
Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992b). Spatial mental models
derived from survey and route descriptions. Journal of Memory
and Language, 31, 261-282.
Taylor, H.A., & Tversky, B. (1996). Perspective in spatial
descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35,
371-391.
Terrace, H.S. (1991). Chunking during serial-learning by a
pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior
Processes, 17(1), 81-93.
Thinus-Blanc, C. (1996). Animal spatial cogntion: Behavioral
and neural approaches. Singapore: World Scientific.
Thorndyke, P.W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial
knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive
Psychology, 14, 560-589.
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men.
Psychological Review, 55, 189-208.
Tolman, E.C., & Honzik, C.H. (1930). Insight in rats.
University of California Publications in Psychology, 4,
215-232.
Tomback, D. (1980). How nutcrackers find their seed stores.Condor,
82, 10-19.
Tomback, D. (1983). Nutcrackers and pines: Coevolution or
coadaptation? In M.H. Nitecki (Ed.), Coevolution
(pp. 179-223). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Tomback, D. (1998). Clark's Nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana).
In A. Poole & F. Gill (Eds.), The birds of North America
(No. 331). Philadelphia, PA: The Birds of North America, Inc.
Tommasi, L., Gagliardo, A., Andrew, R.J., &
Vallortigara, G. (2003). Separate processing mechanisms for
encoding geometric and landmark information in the avian brain.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1695-1702.
Tommasi, L., & Polli, C. (2004).
Representation of two geometric features of the environment in
the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Animal Cognition,
7, 53-59.
Tommasi, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2000).
Searching for the center: Spatial cognition in the domestic
chick (Gallus gallus). Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26,
477-486.
Tommasi, L., Vallortigara, G., & Zanforlin,
M. (1997). Young chicks learn to localize the center of a
spatial environment. Journal of Comparative Physiology A,
180, 567-572.
Touretzky, D.S., & Redish, A.D. (1996).
Theory of rodent navigation based on interacting representations
of space. Hippocampus, 6, 247-270.
Trullier, O., & Meyer, J.A. (1998). Animat navigation using a
cognitive graph. In Blumberg et al. (Eds.). From animals to
animats 5: Proceedings of the fifth international conference on
simulation of adaptive behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Turcek, F.J., & Kelso, L. (1968). Ecological aspects of food
transportation and storage in the Corvidae. Communications in
Behavioral Biology, Part A, 1, 277-297.
Tversky, B. (1993). Cognitive maps, cognitive collages, and
spatial mental models. In A.U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.),
Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS
(pp. 14-24). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Tversky, B., Morrison, J. B., & Betrancourt, M. (2002).
Animation: Can it facilitate? International Journal of
Human-Computer Studies, 57(4), 247-262.
Uttal, D.H. (2000). Seeing the big picture: Map use and the
development of spatial cognition. Developmental Science, 3,
247-286.
Uttal,
D.H., Fisher, J.A., & Taylor, H.A. (2006). Words and maps: Developmental changes in mental models of spatial information.
Developmental Science,
9(2), 221-235.
Vallortigara, G., Pagni, P., & Sovrano, V.A.
(2004). Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial
reorientation: Evidence from a lopsided animal brain. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 390-400.
Vallortigara, G., Zanforlin, M., & Pasti, G.
(1990). Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: A
test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Journal of
Comparative Psychology, 104, 248-254.
Vander Wall, S.B. (1982). An experimental analysis of cache
recovery in Clark's nutcracker. Animal Behaviour 30,
84-94.
Vander Wall, S.B. (1990). Food hoarding in animals.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Vander Wall S.B., & Balda, R.P. (1977). Co-adaptation of the
Clark's nutcracker and pinyon pine for efficient seed harvest
and dispersal. Ecological Monographs, 47, 89-111.
Vander Wall, S.B., & Balda, R.P. (1981). Ecology and evolution
of food-storage behavior in conifer-seed-caching corvids.
Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 56, 217-242.
Vander Wall, S.B., & Hutchins, H.E. (1983). Dependence of
Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) on conifer
seeds during the post-fledging period. Canadian
Field-Naturalist, 97, 208-214.
Vander Wall, S.B., Hoffman, S.W., & Potts, W.K. (1981).
Emigration behavior of Clark's nutcrackers. Condor, 83,
162-170.
Vanderwolf, C.H. (1969). Hippocampal electrical activity and
voluntary movement in the rat. Clinical Neurophysiology, 26,
407-418.
Vargas, J.P., & López,
J.C. (2005). Different ways of encoding geometric information by
goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 119, 458-460.
Vargas, J.P., López, J.C., Salas, C., &
Thinus-Blanc, C. (2004). Encoding of geometric and featural
information by goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal
of Comparative Psychology, 118, 206-216.
Vargas, J.P., Petruso, E.J., & Bingman, V.P.
(2004). Hippocampal formation and geometric navigation in
pigeons. European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1937-1944.
Vogt, B.A., & Miller, M.W. (1983). Cortical connections between
rat cingulate cortex and visual, motor, and postsubicular
cortices. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 216,
192-210.
Voicu, H. (2003). Hierarchical cognitive maps. Neural
Networks, 16, 569-576.
Voicu, H., & Schmajuk, N.A. (2001a). Three-dimensional cognitive
mapping with a neural network. Robotics and Autonomous
Systems, 35, 21-35.
Voicu, H., & Schmajuk, N.A. (2001b). Hierarchical cognitive
maps. Fifth international conference on cognitive and neural
systems, ICCNS '01, Boston, USA.
Voicu H., & Schmajuk, N.A. (2002). Exploration, navigation and
cognitive mapping. Adaptive Behavior, 8(3-4), 207-223.
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M.P. (1995). Magnitude of sex
differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and
consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin,
117, 250-270.
Walcott, C. (1991). Magnetic maps in pigeons. In P. Berthold
(Ed.) Orientation in birds (pp. 38-51).
Basel: Birkhäuser Verlag.
Walcott, C., & Schmidt-Koenig, K, (1973).
The effect on pigeon homing of anesthesia during
displacement. Auk, 90,
281-286.
Waldvogel, J.A. (1987). Olfactory
navigation in homing pigeons: Are the current models
atmospherically realistic? Auk, 104, 369-379.
Waldvogel, J.A., Benvenuti, S., Keeton, W.T., & Papi, F.
(1978). Homing pigeon orientation influenced by deflected winds
at home loft. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 128,
297-301.
Waldvogel, J.A., & Phillips, J.B. (1982). Pigeon
Homing: New experiments involving permanent-resident
deflector-loft birds. In F. Papi, & H.G.
Wallraff (Eds.), Avian Navigation (pp. 179-189). Berlin:
Springer Verlag.
Walker, M., Diebel, C.E., Haugh, C.V., Pankhurst, P.M.,
Montogmery, J.C., & Green, C.R. (1997). Structure and
function of the vertebrate magnetic sense. Nature, 390,
371-376.
Walker, M.M., Dennis, T.E., & Kirschvink, J.L.
(2002). The magnetic sense and its use in long-distance
navigation by animals. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12,
735-744.
Wall, P.L., Botly, L.C.P., Black, C.K., &
Shettleworth, S.J. (2004). The geometric module in the rat:
Independence of shape and feature learning in a food finding
task. Learning & Behavior, 32, 289-298.
Waller, D., Loomis, J. M., & Haun, D. B. M. (2004). Body-based
senses enhance knowledge of directions in large-scale
environments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1),
157-163.
Wallraff, H.G. (1974). Das navigationssystem der Vögel.
Munich: R.Oldenburg.
Wallraff, H.G. (1990). Navigation by homing
pigeons. Ethology, Ecology, & Evolution, 2, 81-115.
Wallraff, H.G. (1999). The magnetic map of homing pigeons: An
evergreen phantom. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 197,
265-269.
Wallraff, H.G. (2001). Navigation by homing pigeons: Updated
perspective. Ethology, Ecology, & Evolution, 13, 1-48.
Wallraff, H.G. (2004). Avian olfactory navigation: Its empirical
foundation and conceptual state. Animal Behaviour, 67,
189-204.
Wallraff, H.G., Kiepenheuer, J., Neumann, M.F., & Streng, A.
(1995). Homing experiments with starlings deprived of the
sense of smell. Condor, 97, 20-26.
Wan, R.Q., Pang, K., & Olton, D.S. (1994). Hippocampal and
amygdaloid involvement in nonspatial and spatial working memory
in rats: Effects of delay and interference. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 108, 866-882.
Wang, R.F. (2004). Between reality and imagination: When is
spatial updating automatic? Perception & Psychophysics, 66(1),
68-76.
Wang, R.F., & Brockmole, J.R. (2003a). Human navigation in
nested environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(3), 398-404.
Wang, R.F., & Brockmole, J.R. (2003b). Simultaneous spatial
updating in nested environments. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 10(4), 981-986.
Wang, R.F., & Spelke, E.S. (2000). Updating egocentric
representations in human navigation. Cognition, 77,
215-250.
Wang, R.F., & Spelke, E.S. (2002). Human
spatial representation: Insights from animals. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 6, 376-382.
Wang, R.F., & Spelke, E.S. (2003).
Comparative approaches to human navigation. In K.J. Jeffery
(Ed.), The neurobiology of spatial behaviour (pp.
119-143). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Warren, S. G., & Juraska, J.M. (1997). Spatial and nonspatial
learning across the rat estrous cycle. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 111, 259-266.
Watson, A. B., & Robson, J.G. (1981). Discrimination at
threshold: Labeled detectors in human vision. Vision
Research, 21, 1115-1122.
Wauchope, K. (1996). Multimodal interaction with a map-based
simulation system (Technical Report No. AIC-96-019).
Washington, D.C.: Naval Research Laboratory.
Webb, B. (2000). What does robotics offer
animal behaviour? Animal Behaviour, 60, 545-558.
Webb, J.K., & Shine, R. (1992). To find an
ant: Trail-following in Australian blindsnakes (Typhlopidae).
Animal Behaviour, 43, 941-948.
Wehner, R. (1998). Navigation in context: Grand theories and
basic mechanisms. Journal of Avian Biology,
29, 370-386.
Weindler, P., Wiltschko, R., & Wiltschko, W. (1996). Magnetic
information affects the stellar orientation of young bird
migrants. Nature, 383, 158-160.
Westcott, P.W. (1964). Invasion of Clark's nutcrackers and
pinyon jays into southeastern Arizona. Condor, 66, 441.
Whishaw, I.Q. (1991). Latent leaning in a
swimming pool place task by rats: Evidence for the use of
associative and not cognitive mapping processes. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B, 83-103.
Whishaw, I.Q., & Gorny, B. (1999). Path integration absent in
scent-tracking fimbria-fornix rats: Evidence for hippocampal
involvement in "sense of direction" and "sense of distance"
using self-movement cues. Journal of Neuroscience, 19,
4662-4673.
Whishaw, I.Q., Hines, D.J., & Wallace, D.G. (2001). Dead
reckoning (path integration) requires the hippocampal formation:
Evidence from spontaneous exploration and spatial learning tasks
in light (allothetic) and dark (idiothetic) tests.
Behavioural Brain Research, 127, 49-69.
Whishaw, I.Q., McKenna, J.E., & Maaswinkel, H. (1997).
Hippocampal lesions and path integration. Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, 7, 228-234.
White, A.R., Strasser, R., & Bingman, V.P. (2002). Hippocampus
lesions impair landmark array spatial learning in homing
pigeons: A laboratory study. Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory, 78, 65-78.
White, E.M., Goncalves, D.M., Partridge, J.C., & Oliveira, R.F.
(2004). Vision and visual variation in the peacock blenny.
Journal of Fish Biology, 65, 227-250.
Wiener, S.I. (1993). Spatial and behavioral correlates of
striatal neurons in rats performing a self-initiated navigation
task. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 3802-3817.
Wilkie, D.M., Willson, R.J., & Kardal, S. (1989). Pigeons
discriminate pictures of a geographic location. Animal
Learning & Behavior, 17, 163-171.
Williams, C.L., Barnett, A. M., & Meck, W.H. (1990).
Organizational effects of gonadal secretions on sexual
differentiation in spatial memory. Behavioral Neuroscience,
104, 84-97.
Williams, C.L., & Meck, W.H. (1991). The organizational effects
of gonadal steroids on sexually dimorphic spatial ability.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 16, 155-176.
Wilson, C.J. (1995). The contribution of cortical neurons to
the firing pattern of striatal spiny neurons. In J.C. Houk, J.L.
Davis, & D.G. Beiser (Eds.), Models of information processing
in the basal ganglia (pp. 29-50). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Wilson, M.A., & McNaughton, B.L. (1993). Dynamics of the
hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science, 261,
1055-1058.
Wilson, S.G., Rinck, M., McNamara, T. P., Bower, G.H.,, &
Morrow, D. G. (1993). Mental models and narrative
comprehension: Some qualifications. Journal of Memory and
Language, 32, 141-154.
Wiltschko, R. (1996). The function of olfactory input in pigeon
orientation: Does it provide navigational information or play
another role? Journal of Experimental Biology, 199(1),
113-119.
Wiltschko, R., & Wiltschko, W. (1981). The development of sun
compass orientation in young homing pigeons. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology, 9, 135 - 141.
Wiltschko, R., Schöps, M., & Kowalski, U. (1989). Pigeon
homing: Wind exposition determines the importance of olfactory
input. Naturwissenschaften, 76, 229-231.
Wiltschko, R., & Wiltschko, W. (1995). Magnetic orientation
in animals. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Wiltschko, R., & Wiltschko, W. (1998). Pigeon homing: Effect of
various wavelengths of light during displacement.
Naturwissenschaften, 85, 164-167.
Wiltschko, R., & Wiltschko, W. (2000). A strategy for beginners!
Reply to Wallraff (2000). Animal Behaviour, 60, F37-F43.
Wiltschko, R., & Wiltschko, W. (2003). Avian navigation: From
historical to modern concepts. Animal Behaviour, 65,
257-272.
Wiltschko, W., & Balda, R.P. (1989). Sun compass orientation in
seed caching scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens).
Journal of Comparative Physiology, 164, 717-721.
Wiltschko, W., Balda, R.P., Jahnel, M., & Wiltschko, R. (1999).
Sun compass orientation in seed-caching corvids: Its role in
spatial memory. Animal Cognition, 2, 215-221.
Wiltschko, W., Daum, P., Fergenbauer-Kimmel, A., & Wiltschko, R.
(1987). The development of the star compass in garden warblers
Sylvia borin. Ethology, 74, 285-292.
Wiltschko, W., & Gwinner, E. (1974). Evidence for an innate
magnetic compass in garden warblers. Naturwissenschaften, 61,
406.
Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H., & Wiltschko, R. (1993). Red
light disrupts magnetic orientation of migratory birds.
Nature, 364, 525-527.
Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Ford, H., & Wiltschko, R.
(2003). Lateralization of magnetic compass orientation in
silvereyes, Zosterops l. lateralis. Australian Journal of
Zoology, 51, 597-602.
Wiltschko, W., Munro, U., Wiltschko, R., & Kirschvink, J.L.
(2002). Magnetite-based magnetoreception in birds: The effect
of a biasing field and a pulse on migratory behavior. Journal
of Experimental Biology, 205(19), 3031-3037.
Wiltschko, W., Traudt, J., Güntürkün, O., Prior, H., & Wiltschko,
R. (2002). Lateralization of magnetic compass orientation in a
migratory bird. Nature, 419, 467-470.
Wiltschko, W., & Wiltschko, R. (1972). Magnetic compass of
European robins. Science, 176, 62-64.
Wiltschko, W., & Wiltschko, R. (1975). The interaction of stars
and magnetic field in the orientation systems of night migrating
birds. II. Spring experiments with European robins (Erithacus
rubecula) Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 39,
265-282.
Wiltschko, W., & Wiltschko, R. (1995). Migratory orientation of
European robins is affected by the wavelength of light as well
as by a magnetic pulse. Journal of Comparative Physiology A,
177, 363-369.
Wiltschko, W., & Wiltschko, R. (1999). The effect of yellow and
blue light on magnetic compass orientation in European robins
Erithacus rubecula. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 184,
295-299.
Wiltschko, W., & Wiltschko, R. (2001). Light-dependent
magnetoreception in birds: The behaviour of European robins,
Erithacus rubecula, under monochromatic light of various
wavelengths and intensities. Journal of Experimental Biology,
204, 3295-3302.
Winson, J. (1984). Neural transmission through the hippocampus:
Dependence on behavioral state. In F. Reinoso-Suarez, & C.
Ajmone-Marsan (Eds.), Cortical integration (pp. 131-146).
New York: Raven.
Winson, J. (1978). Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in
spatial memory deficit in the rat. Science, 201, 160-163.
Wood, E.R., Dudchenko, P.A., Robitsek, R.J., & Eichenbaum, H.
(2000). Hippocampal neurons encode information about different
types of memory episodes occurring in the same location.
Neuron, 27, 623-633.
Yeagley, H.L. (1947). A preliminary study of a physical basis of
bird navigation. Part I. Journal of Applied Physics, 18(12),
1035-1063.
Yeagley, H.L. (1951). A preliminary study of a physical basis of
bird navigation. Part II. Journal of Applied Physics, 22(6),
746-760.
Yeap, W.K., & Jefferies, M.E. (1999).
Computing representations of the local environment.
Artificial Intelligence, 107, 265-301.
Yeshenko, O., Guazzelli, A., & Mizumori, S.J.Y. (2004).
Context-dependent reorganization of spatial and movement
representations by simultaneously recorded hippocampal and
striatal neurons during performance of allocentric and
egocentric tasks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 751-769.
Yeschenko, O.,
Guazzelli, A., & Mizumori, S. J. Y. (2001).
Differential hippocampal neuronal
activity during place or response performance on a T-maze.
Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, Program No. 953.8.
Zentall, T. R.
(1993). Animal cognition: An approach to the study of animal behavior.
In T. R. Zentall (Ed.) Animal cognition: Essays in Honor of Donald A.
Riley (pp. 3-15). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zheng, T., & Wilson, C.J. (2002). Corticostriatal combinatorics:
The implications of corticostriatal axonal arborizations.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 87, 1007-1017.
Zhou, T.L., Tamura, R., Kuriwaki, J., & Ono, T. (1999).
Comparison of medial and lateral septal neuron activity during
performance of spatial tasks in rats. Hippocampus, 9,
220-234.
Zilles, K., & Wree, A. (1995). Cortex: Areal and laminar
structure. In G. Paxinos (Ed.), The rat nervous system
(pp. 649-685). San Diego: Academic.
Zipser, D. (1985). A computational model of hippocampal place
fields. Behavioral Neuroscience, 99, 1006-1018.
Zusi, R.L. (1987). A feeding adaptation of the jaw articulation
in new world jays (Corvidae). Auk, 104, 665-680.
Zwaan, R.A., & Radvansky, G.A. (1998). Situation models in
language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin,
123(2), 162-185.