VII. References
Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components:
A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94,
115-147.
Blough, D. S. (1985). Discrimination
of letters and random dot patterns by pigeons and humans. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 261-280.
Butler, A. B.,& Hodos, W. (1996). Comparative
vertebrate neuroanatomy: Evolution and adaptation. New York, NY: Wiley-Liss.
Cerella, J. (1980). The pigeon's analysis of
pictures.
Pattern Recognition, 9, 1-6.
Cook, R. G. (1993b). The experimental analysis
of cognition in animals.
Psychological Science, 4, 174-178.
Cook, R. G., Cavoto, B. R., Katz, J.,&
Cavoto, K.K. (1997). Pigeon perception and discrimination of rapidly changing
texture stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior
Processes, 23, 390-400.
Cook, R. G.,& Katz, J. S. (1999). Dynamic
object perception in pigeons.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 25, 194-210.
D'Eath, R. B. (1998). Can video images imitate
real stimuli in animal behaviour experiments? Biological Reviews, 73,
267-292.
Dagg, A. I. (1977). The walk of the silver
gull (Larus novaehollandiae) and other birds. Journal of Zoology
(London), 19, 529-540.
Dittrich, W. H. (1999). Seeing biological motion
- Is there a role for cognitive strategies? In A. Braffort, R. Gherbi,
S. Gibet, J. Richardson, & D. Teil (Eds.), Gesture-Based communication
in human-computer interaction: Vol. 1739 (pp. 3-22). Berlin, Germany:
Springer.
Dittrich, W. H., Gilbert, F., Green, P., McGregor,
P.,& Grewcock, D. (1993). Imperfect mimicry: a pigeon's perspective.
Proceedings of the Royal Society London, B251, 195-200.
Dittrich, W. H.,& Lea, S. E. G. (1993).
Motion
as a natural category for pigeons: Generalization and a feature-positive
effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59,
115-129.
Dittrich, W. H., Lea, S. E. G., Barrett, J.,&
Gurr, P.R. (1998). Categorisation of natural movements by pigeons: Visual
concept discrimination and biological motion. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 70, 281-299.
Emmerton, J. (1986). The pigeon's discrimination
of movement patterns (Lissajous figures) and contour-dependent rotational
invariance. Perception, 15, 573-588.
Erichsen, J. T., Hodos, W., Evinger, C., Bessette,
B. B.,& Phillips, S. J. (1989). Head orientation in pigeons: postural,
locomotor and visual determinants. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 33,
268-278.
Erikson, C. W.,& Hoffman, J. E. (1972).
Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual
displays. Perception and Psychophysics, 12, 201-204.
Ewert, J.-P. (1982). Advances in vertebrate
neuroethology.
Trends in Neurosciences, 5, 141-143.
Fersen, L.,& Lea, S. E. G. (1990). Category
discrimination by pigeons using five polymorphous features. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 69-84.
Friedman, M. B. (1975). How birds use their
eyes. In P.Wright, P.G.Caryl & D.M.Vowles (Eds.), Neural and Endocrine
Aspects of Behaviour in Birds (pp. 181-204). Oxford, U.K.: Elsevier.
Frost, B. J. (1978). The optokinetic basis
of head-bobbing in the pigeon.
Journal of Experimental Biology, 74,
187-195.
Frost, B. J.,& DiFranco, C.E. (1976). Motion
characteristics of single units in the pigeon optic tectum. Vision Research,
16, 1229-1234.
Frost, B. J., & Nakayama, K. (1983). Single
visual neurons code opposing motion independent of direction. Science,
220, 744-745.
Green, P. R. (1998). Head orientation and trajectory
of locomotion during jumping and walking in domestic chicks. Brain,
Behavior and Evolution, 51, 48-58.
Green, P. R. (1998). Head orientation is aligned
with take-off trajectory
as chicks jump. Experimental Brain Research,
122, 295-300.
Griffin, D. R., & Thompson, D. (1982).
Echolocation by cave swiftlets. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology,
10, 119-123.
Hayes, B. P., Hodos, W., Holden, A. L., &
Low, J. C. (1987). The projection of the visual field upon the retina of
the pigeon. Vision Research, 27, 31-40.
Hodos, W., Smith, L., & Bonbright, J.C.
(1976). Detection
of the velocity of movement of visual stimuli by pigeons. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25,143-156.
Jassik-Gerschenfeld, D., & Guichard, J.
(1972). Visual receptive fields of single cells in the pigeon's optic tectum.
Brain Research, 40, 303-317.
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of
biological motion and a model of its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics,
14, 201-211.
Kamil, A.C. (1988). A synthetic approach to
the study of animal intelligence. In D.W. Leger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium
on Motivation: Vol. 35. Comparative Perspectives in Modern Psychology
(pp. 257-308). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Katzir, G. (1993). Visual mechanisms of prey
capture in water birds. In H.P. Zeigler & H.-J. Bischof (Eds.), Vision,
Brain, and Behavior in Birds (pp. 301-315). Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press.
Kirkpartick-Steger, K., & Wasserman, E.
A. (1996).
The
what and where of the pigeon's processing of complex visual stimuli.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22,
60-67.
Kirkpatrick-Steger, K., Wasserman, E. A.,&
Biederman, I. (1996).
Effects
of spatial rearrangement of object components on picture-recognition in
pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65,
465-475.
Kirkpatrick-Steger, K., Wasserman, E. A.,&
Biederman, I. (1998).
Effects
of geon deletion, scrambling, and movement on picture recognition in pigeons.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 24,
34-46.
Koenderink, J. J. (1986). Optic flow. Vision
Research, 26, 161-180.
Lea, S. E. G. (1984). In what sense do pigeons
learn concepts? In H.L. Roitblat, T.G. Bever, & H.S. Terrace (Eds.).
Animal cognition (pp. 263-276). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.
Lea, S. E. G., & Dittrich, W. H. (1999).
What do birds see in moving video images? Current Psychology of Cognition,
18, 765-803.
Lea, S. E. G., Slater, A. M., & Ryan, C.
M. E. (1996). Perception of object unity in chicks: a comparison with the
human infant. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 501-504.
Lee, D. N. (1976). A theory of visual control
of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception,
5, 437-459.
Lee, D. N., Davies, M N. O., Green, P. R.,
& van der Weel, F. (1993). Visual control of velocity of approach by
pigeons when landing. Journal of Experimental Biology, 180, 85-104.
Lee, D. N., & Reddish, P. E. (1981). Plummeting
gannets: a paradigm of ecological optics. Nature, 293, 293-294.
Lythgoe, J. N. (1979). The ecology of vision.
Oxford, U.K.: Clarendon Press.
Martin, G. (1999). Optical structure and visual
fields in birds: their relationship with foraging behaviour and ecology.
In S. N. Archer (Ed.), Adaptive mechanisms in the ecology of vision
(pp.
485-508). Bristol, U.K.: Kluwer Academic.
Martin, G. (1999). Visual fields in short-toed
eagles,
Circaetus gallicus (Accipitridae), and the function of binocularity
in birds.
Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 53, 55-66.
Martinoya, C., Le Houezec, J., Bloch, S. (1984).
Participation of eye convergence during feeding: Evidence for frontal binocular
fixation in a lateral-eyed bird. Neuroscience Letters, 45, 335-339.
Mergner, T., & Rosemeier, T. (1998).
Interaction of vestibular, somatosensory and visual signals for postural
control and motion perception under terrestrial and microgravity conditions.
Brain Research Reviews, 28, 118-135.
Mulvanny, P. (1978). Velocity discrimination
by pigeons.
Vision Research, 18, 531-536.
O'Benar, J.-D. (1976). Electrophysiology of
neural units in goldfish optic tectum. Brain Research Bulletin, 1,
529-541
Pisacreta, R. (1982). Stimulus control of the
pigeon's ability to peck a moving target. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 37, 301-309.
Pratt, D. W. (1982). Saccadic eye movements
are coordinated with head movements in walking chickens. Journal of
Experimental Biology, 97, 217-223.
Ryan, C. M. E., & Lea, S. E. G. (1994).
Images of conspecifics as categories to be discriminated by pigeons and
chickens: slides, video tapes, stuffed birds and live birds. Behavioural
Processes, 33, 155-176.
Ryan, C. M.E., Lea, S.E.G., Alklind, A.-S.,&
Dittrich, W.H. (2001) Discrimination of natural movement categories by
chickens using full-light and point-light stimuli. Experimental Psychology
Society, Bristol meeting, April 5-6, 2001.
Sekuler, A. B., Lee, J. A. J., & Shettleworth,
S. J. (1996). Pigeons do not complete partly occluded figures. Perception,
25, 1109-1120.
Shettleworth, S. J. (1993). Where is the comparison
in comparative cognition?
Psychological Science, 4, 179-184.
Siegel, R. K. (1970). Apparent movement detection
in the pigeon.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14,
93-97.
Siegel, R. K. (1971). Apparent movement and
real movement detection in the pigeon: Stimulus generalization. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 16, 189-192.
Sillman, A. J. (1973). Avian Vision. In D.
S. Farner & J. R. King (Eds.),
Avian Biology: Vol. III (pp.
349-383). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Simpson, J. I., Leonard, C. S., & Soodak,
R. E. (1988). The accessory optic system: Analyzer of self-motion. Annals
of the New York Academy of Sciences, 545, 170-179.
Sperry, R. W. (1950). Neural basis of the spontaneous
optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative
Physiological Psychology, 43, 482-489.
Sun, H., & Frost, B. J. (1998). Computation
of different optical variables of looming objects in pigeon nucleus rotundus
neurons. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 296-303.
Suthers, R. A., & Hector, D. H. (1985).
The physiology of vocalization by the echolocating oilbird, Steatornis
caripensis. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Sensory,
Neural and Behavioral Physiology, 156, 243-266.
Treisman, A. (1988). Features and objects:
The fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology., 40A, 201-237.
Treisman, A., Kahneman, D., & Burkell,
J. (1983). Perceptual objects and the cost of filtering. Perception
and Psychophysics, 33, 527-532.
Tresilian, J. R. (1993). Four questions of
time to contact: A critical examination of research on interceptive timing.
Perception, 22, 653-680.
Troje, N. F., & Frost, B. J. (2000). Head-bobbing
in pigeons: How stable is the hold phase? Journal of Experimental
Biology, 203, 935-940.
Uexkuell, J. (1921). Umwelt und innenwelt
der tiere (2nd ed.). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Von Holst, E. (1954). Relations between the
central nervous system and the peripheral organs. British Journal of
Animal Behaviour, 2, 89-94.
Von Holst, E. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950).
Das Reafferenzprinzip.
Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464-476.
Walls, G.L. (1967). The vertebrate eye and
its adaptive radiation. NewYork, NY: Hafner.
Wang, Y.,& Frost, B. J. (1992). Time
to collision is signalled by neurons in the nucleus rotundus of pigeons.
Nature, 356, 236-238.
Wann, J. P. (1996). Anticipating arrival: Is
the tau margin a specious theory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 22, 1031-1048.
Wasserman, E. A., Kirkpatrick-Steger, K., Van
Hamme, L. J., & Biederman, I. (1993). Pigeons are sensitive to the
spatial organization of complex visual stimuli. Psychological Science,
4, 336-341
Watanabe, S., & Ito, Y. (1991). Discrimination
of individuals in pigeons. Bird Behaviour, 9, 20-29.
Wertheimer, M. (1923). Untersuchungen zur Lehre
von der Gestalt, II.
Psychologische Forschung, 4, 301-350.
Wohlschlaeger, A., Jaeger, R., & Delius,
J.D. (1993). Head and eye movements in unrestrained pigeons (Columba
livia). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 313-319.
Wylie, D. R. W., Bischof, W. F., & Frost,
B. J. (1998). Common reference frame for neural coding of translational
and rotational optic flow. Nature, 392, 278-282.
Wylie, D. R. W., Linkenhoker, B., & Lau,
K. L. (1997). Projections of the nucleus of the basal optic root in pigeons
(Columba livia) revealed with biotinylated dextram amine. Journal
of Comparative Neurology, 384, 517-536.