Emmerton References

    Alsop, B. & Honig, W. K. (1991). Sequential stimuli and relative numerosity discriminations in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 386-395.

     Bahr, D. B. & Bekoff, M. (1999). Predicting flock vigilance from simple passerine interactions: modeling with cellular automata. Animal Behaviour, 58, 831-839.

     Balakrishnan, J. D. & Ashby, F. G. (1992). Subitizing: Magical numbers or mere superstition? Psychological Research, 54, 80-90.

     Bekoff, M. (1995). Vigilance, flock size, and flock geometry: information gathering by western evening grosbeaks (Aves, Fringillidae). Ethology, 99, 150-161.

     Boysen, S. T. (1993). Counting in chimpanzees: Nonhuman principles and emergent properties of number. In S. T. Boysen & E. J. Capaldi (Eds.) The development of numerical competence: Animal and human models. ( pp. 39-59). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 

     Boysen, S. T. & Berntson, G. G. (1989). Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, 23-31.

      Boysen, S.T. & Berntson, G.G. (1995). Responses to quantity: perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 82-6.

     Boysen, S. T., Berntson, G. G., Hannan, M. B., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1996). Quantity-based interference and symbolic representations in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 76-86.

     Boysen, S. T. & Capaldi, E. J. (1993). The development of numerical competence: Animal and human models. ( pp. 39-59). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 

     Brannon, E. M. & Terrace, H. S. (1998). Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. Science, 282, 746-749.

     Brannon, E.M. & Terrace, H.S. (2000). Representation of the numerosities 1-9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 31-49.

     Candland, D. K. (1993). Feral children and clever animals: Reflections on human nature. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

     Capaldi, E J. & Miller, D. J. (1988). Counting in rats: Its functional significance and the independent cognitive processes that constitute it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 3-17.

     Davis, H. & Memmott, J. (1982). Counting behavior in animals: A critical evaluation. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 547-571.

     Davis, H. & Pérusse, R. (1988). Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 561-615.

     Dehaene, S. & Changeux, J.-P. (1993). Development of elementary numerical abilities: A neuronal model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 390-407.

     Elgar, M A., Burren, P. J. & Posen, M. (1984). Vigilance and perception of flock size in foraging house sparrows. Passer domesticus L.,  Behaviour, 90, 215-223.

     Emmerton, J. (1998). Numerosity differences and effects of stimulus density on pigeons’ discrimination performance. Animal Learning and Behavior, 26, 243-256.

     Emmerton, J. (2001). Pigeons’ discrimination of color proportion in computer-generated visual displays. Animal Learning and Behavior, 29, 21-35.

     Emmerton, J. & Delius, J. D. (1993). Beyond sensation: Visual cognition in pigeons. In H. P. Zeigler & H.-J. Bischof (Eds.) Vision, brain, and behavior in birds (pp. 377-390). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

     Emmerton, J., Lohmann, A., & Niemann, J. (1997). Pigeons’ serial ordering of numerosity with visual arrays. Animal Learning & Behavior, 25, 234-244.

     Fetterman, J.G. (1993). Numerosity discrimination: Both time and number matter. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 149-164.

     Gelman, R. & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

     Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279-325.

     Gould, S. J. & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London – Series B: Biological Sciences, 205, 581-598.

     Honig, W. K. & Stewart, K. E. (1989). Discrimination of relative numerosity by pigeons. Animal Learning & Behavior, 17, 134-146.

     Honig, W. K. & Stewart, K. E. (1993). Relative numerosity as a dimension of stimulus control: The peak shift. Animal Learning & Behavior, 21, 346-354.

     Kaufman, E. L., Lord, M. W., Reese, T. W. & Volkmann, J. (1949). The discrimination of visual number. American Journal of Psychology, 62, 489-525.

     Keen, R. & Machado, A. (1999). How pigeons discriminate the relative frequency of events. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 72, 151-175.

     Knorn, H.-J. (1987). Visuelle Diskriminationsleistung von Tauben und Menschen beim simultanen Punktmengenvergleich. [Visual discrimination performance of pigeons and humans when simultaneously comparing dot quantities.] Unpublished Diplomarbeit, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, Bochum, Germany.

     Koehler, O. (1941). Vom Erlernen unbenannter Anzahlen bei Vögeln. [On the learning of unnamed numerosities by birds.] Die Naturwissenschaften, 29, 201-218.

     Koehler, O. (1943). "Zähl"-Versuche an einem Kolkraben und Vergleichsversuche an Menschen. ["Counting" experiments on a raven and comparative experiments on humans.] Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 5, 575-712.

     Krebs, J. R. (1978). Optimal foraging: Decision rules for predators. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.) Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (pp. 23-63). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. 

     Krebs, J. R. & McCleery, R. H. (1984). Optimization in behavioural ecology. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.) Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (2nd, ed., pp.91-121). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. 

     Macmillan, N. A. & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

     Meck, W. H. & Church, R. M. (1983). A mode control model of counting and timing processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 320-334.

     Miller, D. J. (1993). Do animals subitize? In S. T. Boysen & E. J. Capaldi (Eds.) The development of numerical competence: Animal and human models. ( pp. 149-169). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 

     Olthof, A., Iden, C. M., & Roberts, W. A. (1997). Judgments of ordinality and summation of number symbols by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23, 325-339.

     Pepperberg, I. M. (1983). Cognition in the African Grey parrot: Preliminary evidence for auditory/vocal comprehension of the class concept. Animal Learning & Behavior, 11, 179-185.

     Pepperberg, I. M. (1987). Evidence for conceptual quantitative abilities in the African Grey parrot: Labeling of cardinal sets. Ethology, 75, 37-61.

     Pepperberg, I. M. (1994). Numerical competence in an African Gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 36-44.

     Porter, J. P. (1904). A preliminary study of the psychology of the English sparrow. The American Journal of Psychology, 15, 313-346.

     Rilling, M. (1993). Invisible counting animals: A history of contributions from comparative psychology, ethology, and learning theory. In S. T. Boysen & E. J. Capaldi (Eds.) The development of numerical competence: Animal and human models. (pp. 3-37). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 

     Roberts, W. A. & Boisvert, M. J. (1998). Using the peak procedure to measure timing and counting processes in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 24, 416-430.

     Roberts, W. A., Macuda, T. & Brodbeck, D. R. (1995). Memory for number of light flashes in the pigeon. Animal Learning & Behavior, 23, 182-188.

     Roberts, W. A. & Mitchell, S. (1994). Can a pigeon simultaneously process temporal and numerical information? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 66-78.

     Shettleworth, S. J. (1984). Learning and behavioural ecology. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.) Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach. (2nd ed. pp. 170-194). Oxford: Blackwell.

     Thorpe, W. H. (1963). Learning and instinct in animals (2nd ed.). London: Methuen.

     Wesley, F. (1961). The number concept: A phylogenetic review. Psychological Bulletin, 58, 420-428.

     Wolfe, J. B. & Kaplon, M. D. (1941). Effect of amount of reward and consummative activity on learning in chickens. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 31, 353-361.

     Xia, L., Siemann, M. & Delius, J. D. (2000). Matching of numerical symbols with number of responses by pigeons. Animal Cognition, 3, 35-43.

     Zeier, H. (1966). Über sequentielles Lernen bei Tauben, mit spezieller Berücksichtigung des "Zähl"-Verhaltens. [About sequential learning by pigeons, with special consideration of "counting" behavior.] Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 23, 161-189.