Proceedings
of the Tenth Annual
International
Conference On Comparative Cognition
Sponsored by the
Comparative Cognition Society*
Radisson Hotel
*
President: Suzanne MacDonald
Secretary:
Marcia Spetch
Treasurer: Ron Weisman
Past
President: Bob Cook
Program Committee: Mike Brown (Chair),
Suzanne MacDonald, Tom Zentall
CO3-2003 Program Summary |
||
|
Time |
Page |
Wednesday |
|
|
Welcome Reception and Check-In |
3:30-5:00 |
6 |
Opening Comments |
6:00-6:10 |
6 |
Memory |
6:15–7:15 |
6 |
Echoic and Auditory Stimulus Discrimination |
7:25–8:10 |
7 |
Cognitive Mechanisms of Spatial Navigation |
8:25-9:50 |
8 |
Thursday |
|
|
Spatial Representation |
1:30-2:35 |
9 |
Associative Processes and Stimulus Control |
2:45-3:30 |
11 |
Counting and Timing |
3:50-5:05 |
11 |
Individual Differences |
5:25-5:55 |
13 |
Stimulus Categories, Classes, and Patterns |
6:05-7:10 |
13 |
Funding Opportunities For Research and Teaching Programs |
7:10-7:25 |
14 |
Reception |
9:30 |
|
Friday Morning |
|
|
Social Cognition I |
8:00-9:00 |
15 |
Stimulus Value, Reinforcement, & Extinction |
9:10-10:10 |
16 |
Stimulus Identity and Same/Different Concept Learning |
10:20-11:20 |
17 |
Friday Afternoon |
|
|
Visual Discrimination Learning |
1:30-2:45 |
19 |
Spatial Learning and Spatial Cues |
2:55-4:20 |
20 |
Problem Solving and Tool Use |
4:40-5:25 |
22 |
Associative Learning and Cue Competition |
5:35-6:40 |
23 |
Banquet (Requires Separate Registration & Fee) |
7:30 |
|
Saturday |
|
|
Business Meeting of The Comparative Cognition Society |
12:30-1:20 |
24 |
Group Photo Shoot |
1:25 |
24 |
Biological Substrates of Cognitive Processes |
1:40-2:40 |
24 |
Natural Selection and Cognition |
2:55-3:45 |
25 |
Social Cognition II |
4:10-5:00 |
25 |
Causal Reasoning |
5:10-6:15 |
26 |
Reception |
8:30 |
|
Presenters
NAME |
|
INSTITUTION |
EMAIL |
PRESENTATION |
Ursula S. |
Anderson |
Georgia Institute of
Technology & Zoo Atlanta |
32 |
|
Francisco |
Arcediano |
Auburn University |
82 |
|
Stephanie |
Babb |
University of Georgia |
sbabb@arches.uga.edu |
6 |
Gordon |
Bauer |
New College of Florida |
bauer@ncf.edu |
4 |
Aaron |
Blaisdell |
UCLA |
7 |
|
Laurie |
Bloomfield |
University of Alberta |
laurie.bloomfield@ualberta.ca |
12 |
Donald |
Blough |
Brown Univ |
Donald_Blough@brown.edu |
49 |
Jean |
Boal |
Millersville University |
jean.boal@millersville.edu |
70 |
Kent |
Bodily |
Auburn University |
59 |
|
Kristin |
Bonnie |
Emory University |
45 |
|
Sarah |
Boysen |
Ohio State University |
101 |
|
Dave |
Brodbeck |
Memorial University of
Newfoundland |
21 |
|
Sarah |
Brosnan |
Emory University |
sbrosna@emory.edu |
94 |
Michael |
Brown |
Villanova University |
19 |
|
Thomas |
Bugnyar |
University of Vermont |
tbugnyar@zoo.uvm.edu |
92 |
Catalin |
Buhusi |
Duke University |
Catalin.buhusi@duke.edu |
29 |
Jackie |
Chappell |
University of Oxford |
78 |
|
Anthony |
Chemero |
Franklin and Marshall
College |
tony.chemero@fandm.edu |
26 |
Russell |
Church |
Brown University |
Russell_Church@Brown.edu |
36 |
Jerome |
Cohen |
University of Windsor |
jcohen@uwindsor.ca |
20 |
Matthew |
Collett |
Michigan State University |
Collettm@msu.edu |
15 |
Robert |
Cook |
Tufts University |
2 |
|
Jonathon |
Crystal |
University of Georgia |
jcrystal@uga.edu |
87 |
Martha |
Escobar |
Auburn University |
83 |
|
Lanny |
Fields |
Queens College - CUNY |
38 |
|
Timothy |
Flemming |
Franklin & Marshall
College |
Timothy.Flemming@Fandm.edu |
57 |
Andrea |
Frank |
University of Iowa |
Andrea-Frank@uiowa.edu |
28 |
Andrea |
Friedrich |
University of Kentucky |
Deafriedrich@aol.com |
8 |
Jennifer |
Gates |
Appalachian State University |
jg36139@appstate.edu |
52 |
Aleida |
Goodyear |
University of Nebraska,
Lincoln |
goodyear@unlserve.unl.edu |
23 |
Kazuhiro |
Goto |
University of Exeter |
K.Goto@exeter.ac.uk |
64 |
Emily |
Gray |
University of Alberta |
Ergray@interbaun.com |
18 |
Paulo |
Guilhardi |
Brown University |
Paulo_Guilhardi@brown.edu |
35 |
Robert |
Hampton |
NIMH |
80 |
|
Heidi |
Harley |
New College of Florida &
Epcot’s Living Seas |
10 |
|
Erica |
Hoy |
University of Georgia |
ehoy@egon.psy.uga.edu |
75 |
Scott |
Husband |
University of South Florida |
88 |
|
Tomoko |
Inagaki |
Hunter College - CUNY |
Tomoko22@aol.com |
33 |
Satoru |
Ishikawa |
Hokkaido University |
ishi_s@complex.eng.hokudai.ac.jp |
39 |
Lucia |
Jacobs |
University of California at
Berkeley |
Jacobs@uclink.berkeley.edu |
91 |
Taylor |
Johnson |
Tufts University |
67 |
|
Alan C. |
Kamil |
U. of Nebraska, Lincoln |
akamil@unlserve.unl.edu |
90 |
Jeffrey |
Katz |
Auburn University |
58 |
|
Richard |
Keen |
Brown University |
30 |
|
Angela |
Kelling |
Georgia Institute of
Technology, Zoo Atlanta |
62 |
|
Debbie |
Kelly |
University of
Nebraska-Lincoln |
17 |
|
Emily |
Klein |
University of Kentucky |
edklei0@uky.edu |
5 |
Angie |
Koban |
Tufts University |
65 |
|
Valerie |
Kuhlmeier |
Yale University |
valerie.kuhlmeier@yale.edu |
76 |
Shannon |
Kundey |
Yale University |
shannon.kundey@yale.edu |
27 |
Olga |
Lazareva |
University of Iowa |
97 |
|
Kang |
Lee |
Queen’s University |
kang@psyc.queensu.ca |
46 |
Katherine |
Leighty |
University of Georgia |
kleighty@arches.uga.edu |
41 |
Jody |
Lewis |
University of Nebraska
Lincoln |
Jlewis@unlserve.unl.edu |
72 |
Chuck |
Locurto |
College of the Holy Cross |
37 |
|
Suzanne |
MacDonald |
York University |
1 |
|
Mika |
MacInnis |
Brown University |
34 |
|
Hiroshi |
Makino |
Chiba University |
hmakino@Cogsci.L.Chiba-u.ac.jp |
66 |
Helena |
Matute |
Universidad de Deusto |
50 |
|
Tammy |
McKenzie |
University of Western
Ontario |
tmckenzi@uwo.ca |
74 |
Ralph R. |
Miller |
SUNY-Binghamton |
rmiller@binghamton.edu |
81 |
Britta |
Osthaus |
University of Exeter, UK |
b.osthaus@ex.ac.uk |
100 |
Linda |
Parker |
Wilfrid Laurier University |
lparker@wlu.ca |
86 |
Sarah |
Partan |
University of South Florida |
93 |
|
Tadd |
Patton |
University of South Florida |
48 |
|
Guillermo |
Paz-y-Mińo C. |
University of
Nebraska-Lincoln |
pazymino@unlserve.unl.edu |
43 |
Matthew |
Pizzo |
University of Georgia |
22 |
|
Tyson |
Platt |
Auburn University |
84 |
|
Amy |
Pollick |
Emory University |
apollic@emory.edu |
47 |
Mary Jo |
Rattermann |
Franklin & Marshall
College |
56 |
|
William |
Roberts |
University of Western
Ontario |
Roberts@uwo.ca |
68 |
Karen L. |
Roper |
Wake Forest University |
roperk@wfu.edu |
54 |
Carrie |
Rosengart |
University of Georgia |
9 |
|
James |
Rowan |
Bridgewater College |
jrowan@bridgewater.edu |
40 |
Nestor |
Schmajuk |
Duke University |
25 |
|
Bennett L. |
Schwartz |
Florida International
University |
schwartb@fiu.edu |
3 |
Sara J. |
Shettleworth |
University of Toronto |
Shettle@psych.utoronto.ca |
14 |
Shannon |
Skov-Rackette |
University of Toronto |
Shannon.Skov.Rackette@Utoronto.Ca |
24 |
Denise |
Smith |
Kent State University |
dpsmith@kent.edu |
89 |
Michael |
Snyder |
University of Alberta |
mrsnyder@ualberta.ca |
16 |
Marcia |
Spetch |
University of Alberta |
mspetch@ualberta.ca |
61 |
Janice |
Steirn |
Georgia Southern University |
51 |
|
Fred |
Stollnitz |
National Science Foundation |
42 |
|
Jennifer |
Sutton |
University of Toronto |
60 |
|
Bridgette |
Szekeres |
University of Western
Ontario |
71 |
|
Sabine |
Tebbich |
University of Cambridge |
77 |
|
Sayaka |
Tsutsumi |
Kyoto University |
stsutsumi@bun.kyoto-u.ac.jp |
96 |
Claudia |
Uller |
University of Louisiana –
Lafayette |
uller@louisiana.edu |
44 |
Jennifer |
Vonk |
University of Louisiana at
Lafayette |
Jxv9592@louisiana.edu |
31 |
Edward |
Wasserman |
University of Iowa |
98 |
|
Shigeru |
Watanabe |
Keio University |
swat@flet.keio.ac.jp |
85 |
Cynthia |
Wei |
Michigan State University |
weicynth@msu.edu |
73 |
Alex |
Weir |
Department of Zoology,
Oxford University |
79 |
|
Ronald |
Weisman |
Queen’s University |
ron@psyc.queensu.ca |
11 |
Whitney |
Werstlein |
Appalachian State University |
ww38372@appstate.edu |
53 |
Bill |
Whitlow |
Rutgers University |
bwhitlow@camden.rutgers.edu |
99 |
Robert |
Willson |
University of Greenwich |
R.Willson@gre.ac.uk |
95 |
Tony |
Wright |
University of Texas Medical
School at Houston |
anthony.a.wright@uth.tmc.edu |
55 |
Clive |
Wynne |
University of Florida |
wynne@ufl.edu |
69 |
Kumiko |
Yokoyama |
Hunter College, CUNY |
63 |
|
Shaowu |
Zhang |
Australian National
University |
swzhang@rsbs.anu.edu.au |
13 |
Co-Authors and Discussants
NAME |
|
INSTITUTION |
EMAIL |
|
|
|
|
Robert |
Batsell |
Kalamazoo College |
|
John |
Batson |
Furman University |
|
Shelley |
Batts |
|
|
Robert |
Biegler |
NTNU |
|
Sheila |
Chase |
Hunter College – CUNY |
|
Tricia |
Clement |
University of Kentucky |
|
Jennifer |
Davis |
University of Southern
Mississippi |
|
James |
Denniston |
Appalachian State University |
|
Steve |
Fountain |
Kent State University |
|
Jennifer |
Fugate |
Emory University |
|
Leonard |
Green |
Washington University of St.
Louis |
|
Eric |
Heinemann |
Hunter College - CUNY |
|
Michelle |
Hernandez |
Auburn University |
|
Sharon |
Himmanen |
Lehman College - CUNY |
|
Megan |
Hoffman |
Florida International
University |
|
Brooke |
Huntley |
|
|
Miranda |
Karson |
Michigan State University |
|
Lauren |
Lanza |
Franklin & Marshall
College |
|
Kristen |
Keller |
Franklin & Marshall
College |
|
Radhika |
Makecha |
CUNY |
|
Tamo |
Nakamura |
University of Texas Medical
School at Houston |
|
Shannon |
Rackette |
|
|
Amanda |
Reaves |
Auburn University |
|
Jacquelyne |
Rivera |
University of Texas Medical
School at Houston |
Jacquelyne.J.Rivera@uth.tmc.edu |
Rosalind |
Sadleir |
University of Florida |
|
Brosnan |
Sarah |
Emory University |
|
Todd |
Schachtman |
University of Missouri |
|
Toru |
Shimizu |
University of South Florida |
shimizu@chuma1.cas.usf.edu |
Christopher |
Sturdy |
University of Alberta |
|
Francys |
Subial |
|
|
Peter |
Sugarman |
University of Southern
Mississippi |
|
William |
Suits |
Georgia Southern University |
|
Karyl |
Swartz |
CUNY |
|
Rachel |
Thames |
University of Southern
Mississippi |
|
Kristen |
Totonelly |
Franklin & Marshall
College |
|
Roger |
Thompson |
Franklin & Marshall
College |
|
Sylvana |
Yelda |
|
Opening Comments
1-10 Comparative Cognition: Past, present and future
Suzanne
MacDonald (York University : President, Comparative Cognition Society)
Memory
Chair, Suzanne MacDonald
6:00
2-10 Mechanisms of recognition and
recall in pigeons
Robert Cook, Richard Chechile, & Shelley Roberts
(Tufts University)
Testing concurrent recognition
and a recall-analog tasks, the short-term memory of four pigeons for compound
stimuli comprised of two colored blocks was tested. Using multinomial process
tree modeling for obtaining estimates of successful retrieval, sufficient
storage, partial storage, and no storage, the effects of delay were examined in
the two tasks. The effects of delay were most pronounced on the storage
estimates of retention in each bird, while the retrieval process seemed little
affected by time. Implications for the modeling of avian memory will be
discussed.
6:15
3-10 Trial-unique
learning and episodic-like memory in gorillas
Bennett L. Schwartz , Christian A. Meissner (Florida
International University), Sian Evans (DuMond Conservancy), Megan Hoffman,
& Leslie D. Frazier (Florida International University)
Trial-unique learning and
episodic-like memory were examined in an adult male gorilla (Gorilla gorilla
gorilla). We addressed if gorillas
can retrieve and communicate information about the past. The gorilla witnessed a series of unique
events, involving a familiar person (Experiment 1), an unfamiliar person
(Experiment 2), or a novel object (Experiment 3). Following a five to ten-minute retention
interval, a tester gave the gorilla three photographs mounted on wooden cards. One of these photographs represented the
target object or person of the event, whereas the other two were
distractors. The gorilla responded by
handing back a particular photograph. If
correct, he was rewarded with food. In
all three of these experiments, the gorilla was significantly above chance at
remembering the target. In Experiment
4, at a 24-hour retention interval, the gorilla was not significantly different
from chance. The results are interpreted
in light of theories of episodic memory.
6:30
4-5 The serial position effect in
honeybees
Gordon B. Bauer, Prentiss L. McNeill (New College of
Florida), & Shelley A. Batts
Twenty-five honeybees, Apis
mellifera, were trained individually in a five position, color-cued, forced
choice sequence over 15 trials. On every
trial a subject fed from each of five differently colored targets arranged in a
pentagon formation. Each subject was
trained on a single color/location sequence, counterbalanced across
subjects. Memory was measured by a ten-minute
extinction test in which all contacts with targets were counted. Primacy and recency effects were demonstrated
as indicated by a significant quadratic trend, indicating a greater numbers of
contacts with targets at the beginning and end of the sequences. No preference effects were observed for the
color/location of targets independent of position order. Although the number of errors decreased
significantly over learning trials, no memory for sequences was indicated
during extinction tests.
6:35
5-5 Sample matching in pigeons:
Divergent retention functions may result from ambiguity between delay and
inter-trial interval
Emily D. Klein & Thomas R. Zentall (University of
Kentucky)
Gaitan & Wixted (2000)
found that when 0- and 10-sec samples were associated with one comparison and a
2-sec sample was associated with a different comparison, pigeons appeared to
respond based on the presence versus the absence of the 2-sec sample. As a
result, a choose-long bias was observed. However, it is also possible that the
pigeons were demonstrating a “choose-nothing” bias due to confusion between the
dark inter-trial interval (ITI) and the dark delay interval (DI). The present
experiment explored this hypothesis by clearly distinguishing between the ITI
and the DI. Consistent with results obtained by Gaitan & Wixted (2000),
pigeons in the Dark-Dark group demonstrated a clear choose long effect.
However, pigeons in the Lit-Dark group demonstrated symmetrical forgetting for
all sample durations across delays.
6:40
6-5 Episodic-like memory in the rat
Stephanie J. Babb & Jonathon D. Crystal (University
of Georgia)
We investigated episodic-like
memory in rats (n = 5) using an eight-arm radial maze. The rats received daily
training consisting of forced-choice visits to four baited arms; one randomly
chosen arm contained chocolate each day. The second phase consisted of the
availability of all eight arms. After a short (30 min) retention interval (RI),
the four arms that were not available in Phase 1 provided food. After a long (4
hr) RI, the four remaining arms plus the arm containing chocolate provided food
(i.e., the chocolate arm replenished). The rats visited the chocolate location
after the long RI more than after the short RI. Next, chocolate was paired with
lithium chloride, and subsequent testing used the long RI. The rats visited the
chocolate location less after the taste aversion manipulation than in previous
training, demonstrating that the rats had knowledge of what, when, and where
components of episodic memory.
6:45 Discussion: Presentations
4, 5, & 6
6:50
7-5 Assessing the content of
retrospective and prospective memory in rats using a 12-arm radial maze.
Aaron P. Blaisdell & Laura H. Corbit (UCLA)
To investigate the content of
working memory, we used a variant of the 12-arm radial maze procedure of Cook,
Brown, & Riley (1985). Access to a
different set of 4 of the 12 arms was blocked on each trial, while the
remaining 8 arms each contained a single food pellet. On interpolation trials, rats were removed
from the maze after their 2nd, 4th, or 6th arm
entry and were returned 15 minutes later to deplete it. In addition, on occasional probe trials, rats
returned to the maze after the delay were provided with only two open arms, one
of which had been blocked prior to the delay.
The other test arm was a previously open arm that the rat had not yet
entered or one that the rat had entered prior to the delay. These tests interrogate the content of
retrospective and prospective memory.
6:55
8-5 Differential outcomes based on
feeder location can serve as an effective cue for comparison choice
Andrea M. Friedrich & Thomas R. Zentall (University
of Kentucky)
Pigeons were trained on two
identity matching tasks (hues and lines) with differential outcomes based on
feeder location (a correct response following one sample from each task
resulted in presentation of the left feeder, whereas a correct response
following the other sample from each task resulted in presentation of the right
feeder). Transfer tests involving samples from one task (e.g., hues) and
comparisons from the other (e.g., lines) produced positive transfer. Thus, the
expectation of reinforcement at different locations can serve as an effective
choice cue.
7:00
9-5 The A-not-B error in object
permanence: recognition and recall errors in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)
Carrie R. Rosengart & Dorothy M. Fragaszy
(University of Georgia)
According to Piaget, human
infants at a certain stage of object permanence can correctly find an object at
an original (A) location, but continue to search at A when the object is hidden
at a different (B) spot. Eight capuchin monkeys were tested to determine if
they show the A-not-B error and if it is expressed differently in a recognition
task (lifting a cup) versus a recall task (digging in a sandbox). Additionally,
the delay period was manipulated so that the B trials were either 10s (the same
as the delay on A trials) or 30s in order determine the role of memory in the
A-not-B error. The monkeys showed the A-not-B error in both the recognition and
recall conditions, but only when the time delay was longer on the B trials than
on the A trials. The A-not-B error may be accounted for by memory demands.
7:05 Discussion: Presentations
7, 8, & 9
Echoic and Auditory Stimulus Discrimination
Chair, Tony Wright
7:25
10-10 Echoic object feature extraction
by a bottlenose dolphin
Heidi E. Harley (New College of Florida; Epcot’s Living
Seas)
Echolocating dolphins extract
object feature information from echoes, but the features that are extracted
have not been clearly defined. In the
current study, a dolphin performed a three-alternative delayed
matching-to-sample task in which objects that varied on single dimensions
(material, texture, shape, size) were presented in a variety of conditions
(cross-modally: visual sample to echoic alternatives, echoic sample to visual
alternatives; intra-modally: visual sample to visual alternatives, echoic
sample to echoic alternatives), and performance across dimensions and
conditions was analyzed. Object material
and texture appeared to be discriminated mostly echoically, however,
information about shape and size appeared to be extracted by both sensory
systems.
7:45
11-10 Sorting absolute pitches: Are
humans special?
Mitchel Williams, Jerome Cohen (University of Windsor),
& Ronald Weisman (Queen’s University)
The absolute pitch ranges in
birdsongs and human speech can provide cues for recognition. When songbirds and
humans are trained to sort contiguous pitches into 3 or 8 ranges, based on
associations between the ranges and reward and nonreward (see Weisman et al.,
1998), (a) songbirds discriminated 3 ranges to a much higher standard than
humans, and (b) songbirds discriminated 8 ranges with precision whereas humans
acquired only a crude discrimination of the lowest and highest of 8 ranges.
Here, we tested another mammal, Rattus
norvegicus, and found that rats’ discriminations of absolute pitch ranges
are highly similar to those of humans. The results suggest that songbirds are
superior to mammals in their use of absolute pitch to sort frequencies into
ranges.
8:00
12-5 The Chick-a-dee call of the
Mountain Chickadee (Poecile gambeli)
Laurie L. Bloomfield (University of
Alberta), D. Archibald McCallum (Applied Bioacoustics) & Christopher B.
Sturdy (University of Alberta)
All chickadees produce a call
commonly referred to as the ‘chick-a-dee’ call. Because of this and other
similarities in vocal repertoires, chickadees have been the subject of
extensive comparative research. A common nomenclature for calls and call note types
would facilitate the comparison of vocalizations among chickadee species. The
vocalizations of the black-capped chickadee are the most well studied of any
chickadee and could serve as a common reference for the classification of
vocalizations (e.g., calls and note types) produced by other chickadees. Here
we (1) classify the call notes in mountain chickadees’ chick-a-dee calls into
types using the note categories found in black-capped chickadee’s chick-a-dee
calls as a guide and (2) provide a detailed bioacoustic analysis of mountain
chickadee chick-a-dee calls, thus providing a solid foundation for further
comparative studies of chickadee vocalizations.
8:05 Discussion: Presenation
12
Cognitive Mechanisms of Spatial Navigation
Chair, Bill Roberts
8:25
13-20 Small Brains and Smart Minds
S.W. Zhang (Australian National
University) & M.V. Srinivasan (Australian National
University)
The brain of the honeybee has a
volume of about a cubic millimetre, weighs less than a milligram, and carries fewer
than a million neurons. However, recent research is revealing that insects, and
in particular bees, may not be the simple, reflexive creatures that they were
once assumed to be. Over the past few
years we have explored honeybees’ learning capability and examined whether
honeybees can learn strategies or principles for solving complex tasks. In this
short review paper we summarise recent progresses in our laboratory in study of
honeybees learning to negotiate mazes, learning delayed matching-to-sample
tasks (DMTS), learning concept and so on. The evidence clearly showed that i).
The information stored in honeybees’ memory actively participates in visual
information processing; ii). Honeybees are able to learn rules to negotiate
maze; iii). Honeybees are able to group stimuli and to form concept. iv).
Honeybees do communicate food source information by waggle dances.
8:50
14-10 Homing with dead reckoning and
visual cues by food-carrying rats: tests of competition between spatial
learning mechanisms
Sara J. Shettleworth & Jennifer E. Sutton
(University of Toronto)
We tested the notion suggested
by some ethological literature that dead reckoning (path integration) is
employed continuously, in parallel with learning stable visual spatial cues.
Rats searched for pieces of food randomly scattered in a large circular
cue-controlled arena and carried them straight back to a dark home box
concealed behind one of 16 identical doors on the periphery. For each session
of five trips into the arena, the home was relocated and the rats were
disoriented, forcing them to home by dead reckoning with respect to the home
and/or by using a visual cue in a consistent position relative to the home door
(a beacon or a landmark). Consistent
with other research, rats trained with a visual cue homed just as accurately
when the cue was removed (i.e., by dead reckoning). We also tested whether extensive dead
reckoning experience blocks learning to home with a visual cue.
9:05
15-10 Do familiar landmarks reset the global path integration system of desert
ants?
M.Collett (Michigan State University),
T.S.Collett (University of Sussex), S.Chameron (Université Paris Nord) & R. Wehner (University of Zürich)
It is often suggested that animals may link landmark
memories to a global co-ordinate system provided by path integration, thereby
obtaining a map-like representation of familiar terrain. Experiments were
performed to test whether desert ants (Cataglyphis fortis) recall a long-term
memory of a global path integration vector on arriving at a familiar food-site.
Ants were trained to a feeder along L-shaped routes within open channels that
obscured all natural landmarks, but which were marked with conspicuous
artificial landmarks. The homeward vectors of ants accustomed to the route were
tested after a foodward route either as in training or with the length of the
first leg altered. If a familiar route or food-site triggers the recall of an
accustomed home vector, the home vectors would be the same. The actual home
vector reflected the immediately preceding outward journey, suggesting that
landmark memories do not prime the recall of long-term global path integration
memories.
9:20
16-10 Avoiding the middle route in a
spatial navigation task in carp
Michael R. Snyder & Alexza Rojas (University of
Alberta)
When faced with a linear array
of items, humans show a tendency to select an item from the middle of the
array. Christenfeld (1995) suggests this "centrality premise" is
adopted because choosing a middle option requires less cognitive effort,
whereas Shaw et al. (2000) advocate an attentional explanation. Regardless, the
centrality premise is regularly violated by humans engaging in pencil-and-paper
and real-world spatial navigation tasks. When faced with otherwise equivalent
routes going to the same place accessible off one linear path, subjects avoid
the middle option(s), picking either the first or last route. By way of a
cross-species comparison, we tested the centrality premise with goldfish in a
water maze. Starting in one corner of the maze the goldfish had to reach the
goal-box in the diagonally opposite corner by travelling along one of three
equidistant routes. Goldfish avoid the middle route in the maze, showing
behavioural agreement with human results.
9:35
17-5 Effects of goal-landmark distance
on error by Clark's nutcrackers
Debbie M. Kelly & Alan C. Kamil (University of Nebraska, Lincoln)
Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) use spatial memory
to relocate previously cached food items. Several experiments have shown that
nutcrackers use landmarks surrounding the cache site for successful cache
retrieval. The relationship between a
landmark and goal location, such as a cache site, may be defined in terms of
distance and directional information.
The purpose of our experiment was to measure the effects of
goal-landmark distance on search accuracy for distance and direction
estimation. Three groups of nutcrackers were trained to locate a hidden goal at
a fixed distance due north of a single landmark. The groups differed in goal-landmark distance
(i.e., 40, 90 and 140 cm). We
hypothesized that search accuracy should be lowest for the group with the
largest goal-landmark distance and that distance error should increase more
rapidly than directional error as goal-landmark distance increases.
9:40
18-5 Thinking inside the box: Pigeons encode relative distance from walls
Emily R. Gray, Marcia L. Spetch, & Angela Nguyen
(University of Alberta)
Recent studies have
investigated whether animals use absolute or relative distances to find a
hidden goal. In several previous
studies, pigeons have been trained to find a goal in the center of a square
array of four identical landmarks. When
subsequently tested with an expanded array, pigeons did not search in the
center but instead searched at locations that maintained the same absolute
distance from the landmarks as in training.
In this study, pigeons were trained to search for food in the center of
an enclosed square arena designed to block external cues. When tested in a larger arena, the pigeons
predominantly searched the center of the enclosure, indicating that they had
learned the relative distances from the walls to locate the center. Apparently, encoding of spatial location from
walls of an enclosure differs from encoding of spatial location from discrete
landmarks.
9:45 Discussion of
Presentations 17 & 18
Thursday Afternoon
Spatial Representation
Chair, Robert Cook
1:30
19-10 Patterns of spatial pattern exemplars II
Michael F. Brown, Sara Fabian, & Kelly DiGian
(Villanova University)
A previous experiment (Brown,
DiGian, & Smith, CO3-2002) showed that rats can be controlled by a serial
pattern of spatial pattern exemplars – sessions of three trials in which a
patterned sequence of rows of locations in a 5 X 5 matrix of locations is
baited. The present experiment
replicated and extended this result.
However, although there was clear evidence that both the (within-trial)
row pattern and the (across-trials) pattern of rows controlled choices, there was
no evidence that the pattern of rows was extrapolated beyond the domain in
which the rats were trained.
1:45
20-10 Spatial arm configurations and
rats’ performance in the enclosed radial maze: Maps and lists
Jerome Cohen & Joseph Tremblay (University of
Windsor)
When forced to sample three of
four arms in the radial maze (study segment), rats learn and maintain accurate
performance in finding the final arm (test segment) when the relative
configuration of arm cues remain constant than varied. Rats initially acquired this task with two
different sets of four arm cues (full arm inserts; objects at doorways) and
then were able to learn four different configurations when adjacent and
opposite pairs of arm cues from each set were combined into new configurations. Post acquisition varying of two of these
configurations did not affect accuracy within the constant configurations. Over training, however, rats increased their
accuracy on the randomly varied configurations suggesting that they had
developed a list-like representation of those two sets of arm cues.
2:00
21-10 A purely geometric module in
human spatial cognition?
David R. Brodbeck (Sir Wilfred Grenfell College –
Memorial University of Newfoundland) Andrea E. Pike (Memorial University of
Newfoundland) & B.Cory Spracklin (Sir Wilfred Grenfell College – Memorial
University of Newfoundland)
A series of experiments were
conducted to test whether humans use a purely geometric module in spatial
cognition. Based on Cheng’s (1986) work
on rats, subjects were shown a rectangle on a computer screen. They were shown
a goal (suing a dot) and asked to touch the screen. Once they had touched the screen the
rectangle rotated a number of times as the dot (goal) faded. Subjects were then required to point to where
the goal was before the rotation. Tests
included cued and uncued rectangles as well as squares. Results are compared to Cheng (1986) and
recent work with humans.
2:15
22-5 Time-place learning in the
eight-arm radial maze
Matthew J. Pizzo & Jonathon D. Crystal (University
of Georgia)
Rats (n=4) searched for food
on an eight-arm radial maze during daily 56-min sessions. Sessions were divided into eight 7-min time
zones, during which a different location provided food; locations were
randomized across subjects before training.
Rats obtained multiple pellets within each time zone by leaving and
returning to the correct location after a 10-sec delay. A high rate of responding occurred at active
locations. We tested the hypothesis that
rats had knowledge about the temporal and spatial features of the task. Anticipatory visits occurred at each location
before it became active. On occasional
probe sessions, one of the locations was inactive (i.e., no food was
available). The inactive location had
more visits than other locations. More
visits were made to locations that had not yet become active relative to those
that already provided food
2:20
23-5 Clark’s nutcrackers’ (Nucifraga
columbiana) use of absolute and relative bearings in spatial search
Aleida J. Goodyear & Alan C. Kamil (University of
Nebraska, Lincoln)
The Kamil-Cheng multiple
bearings hypothesis emphasizes the role of directional information, or
bearings, in the use of landmarks while locating a hidden goal. Absolute bearings use the compass direction
from the goal to each landmark while relative bearings are measured as the
angle between two landmarks as seen from the goal location. When a landmark array is rotated, relative
bearings remain the same, but absolute bearings are altered. Three groups of nutcrackers were trained with
the same five landmark array, but differing degrees of rotation, allowing us to
test for the use of absolute and relative bearings.
2:25
24-5 Geometric knowledge in the rat: Tests with
exploratory behaviour
Shannon I. Skov-Rackette & Sara J. Shettleworth (University of Toronto)
Animals may acquire spatial
knowledge through exploration and reveal this knowledge by renewing exploration
when objects are moved. Previous research has demonstrated increased exploration to
novel landmark positions, but most experiments have failed to control for
factors such as orientation within the larger, extra-arena environment or use
of non-geometric intramaze cues, which may indicate environmental positional
changes. In a novel paradigm,
disoriented rats encountered one or more objects while searching for randomly
scattered food in a large arena that was devoid of landmarks. Their investigation of single objects, which
underwent positional changes, showed that they encoded the location of such
objects relative to the arena. A series
of experiments asked whether rectangular arrays are encoded as a single
arrangement, or as four independent components, and examined the rat’s
geometric capabilities to make positional judgments.
2:30 Discussion of
Presenations 22, 23, & 24
Associative Processes and Stimulus Control
Chair, Ralph Miller
2:45
25-10 A neural network model of
pre-pulse inhibition
Nestor Schmajuk (Duke University) & Roger Smith
(Cisco)
Pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) refers
to the inhibition of a startle reflex (R) produced by preceding the startling
stimulus (S) with a weak pre-pulse (PP). We offer a formal theory of PPI in the
form of a real-time neural network model. The network assumes that attention to
S is proportional to total novelty, which is defined as the sum of the absolute
value of the difference between the predicted and observed amplitudes of all
environmental events. Attention to S determines the size of R. The model
describes PPI because total novelty and, therefore, attention to the S
decreases after presentation of the PP. This explanation is similar to that
offered for latent inhibition by a related neural network model. Computer
simulations demonstrate that the neural network correctly describes most of the
behavioral properties of PPI.
3:00
26-10 A neural network model of entropy
discrimination in pigeons
Christopher Silansky & Anthony Chemero (Franklin
and Marshall College)
A series of simulation
experiments show that a three-layer, feedforward artificial neural network
(ANN) can model the entropy discrimination abilities of pigeons. A training routine was developed for ANNs
based closely on the training regimes Young and Wasserman used with real
pigeons. Initially, the training routine
was applied to two-layer ANN. The
two-layer ANN was able to make discriminations based on entropy levels in
arrays of 16 icons, performing perfectly on the set of training arrays and at
levels well above chance on novel arrays.
Unlike pigeons, however, the two-layer ANN was unable to subsequently
learn to discriminate among entropy levels in smaller arrays. A computationally more powerful three-layer
ANN was able to make entropy discriminations in arrays of 16 icons, and subsequently
did learn to make entropy discriminations in smaller arrays.
3:15
27-5 Further evaluations of elemental
versus configural accounts of Pavlovian conditioning
Shannon M.A. Kundey & Allan R. Wagner (Yale
University)
Two experiments are reported
relevant to the continuing debate between elemental versus configural accounts
of associative learning. Using eyeblink
conditioning in the rabbit, there was greater generalization between components
and compounds of components than predicted by the configural theory of Pearce
(1987), and than observed in similarly-designed prior studies of autoshaping in
the pigeon. The data from the two
experiments are consistent with the elemental account of Rescorla and Wagner
(1972). The difference in results from
those previously observed in autoshaping are consistent with other data from
the two situations, and consistent with the replaced-element account offered by
Wagner (2001).
3:20
28-5 Associative symmetry in the
pigeon
Andrea J. Frank & Edward A. Wasserman (University
of Iowa)
If an organism is explicitly
taught an A®B association, then might
it also spontaneously acquire the symmetrical B®A association?
Remarkably little evidence attests to such “associative symmetry.” Using a symbolic go/no go matching-to-sample procedure,
we report for the first time a successful case of associative symmetry in the
pigeon. Indeed, the strength of the B®A association can actually equal that of the A®B association.
Our success may be due to the fact that the single-key go/no procedure
avoids confounding the spatial locations of the sample and choice stimuli in
choice matching procedures. We avoided
an analogous temporal confounding with the symbolic go/no go procedure by
having the A and B stimuli serve as both sample and test stimuli on intermixed
identity matching trials. We plan to
extend this research to include training and testing for transitivity, thereby
assessing the species generality of stimulus equivalence, as defined by Murray Sidman.
3:25 Discussion of
Presentations 27 & 28
Counting and timing
Chair, Jonathon Crystal
3:50
29-20 Time and time again: An
integrative approach to interval timing
Catalin V. Buhusi & Warren H. Meck (Duke
University)
The fine structure of the
internal clock can be revealed by examining a paradigm in which rats (Rattus norvegicus), mice (Mus musculus) or pigeons (Columba livia) have to filter out the
gaps (breaks) that (sometimes) interrupt timing. Several lines of study in our lab will be
discussed. First, results suggest that
“stop”/”reset” mechanism of interval timing is controlled by sharing of
attention. Secondly, attention sharing
was found to be affected by dopaminergic drugs, hippocampal lesions, and
genetic manipulations. Finally,
computational modeling examined whether time is represented as a unique value
or as a distributed pattern. The latter
view predicts that the effect of a gap depends on the ratio between the
duration of the interruption and the criterion interval. This prediction was confirmed experimentally
in the above species.
4:15
30-10 Reexamination
of interpretations of memory for temporal intervals
Richard Keen & Russell Church (Brown University)
Does memory for stimulus
duration change as a function of retention interval? Twelve rats were presented with short (2s)
and long (10s) stimuli and reinforced with left and right lever presses,
respectively. Testing consisted of
inserting delays between the stimulus-termination and choice. An initial choose-long effect was observed
(i.e., performance on long trials deteriorated less than on short trials as a
function of delay). After extended
exposure to the testing trials, the choose-long effect disappeared. Two hypotheses are tested against the
data. The first assumes that the animal
learns the duration of the signal and that the memory for that duration decays,
or shortens, over time (i.e., subjective shortening). The second assumes that the animal learns
multiple durations (signal-onset to –termination, signal-onset to food, etc.)
and then uses these durations to make a choice.
4:30
31-5 Numerical Estimation by an
Orangutan (Pongo abelii) in a Matching Task
Jennifer Vonk (University of Louisiana at Lafayette)
An adult male orangutan (Pongo abelii) was presented with a series
of delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS) tasks in which he was to match images
based on a) the number of individuals depicted in the photograph (from 1-4), b)
the number of abstract shapes presented in the stimulus (from 1-4), or c) the
number of dots presented in the stimulus (from 1-4, 4-7, and 7-10). The size of
the dots was manipulated to control for overall ratio of foreground to
background. In addition, the spatial arrangement of the dots and the background
color of the stimuli varied within each set of stimuli depicting the same
number of dots. The orangutan showed a high degree of transfer to novel
numerosities and performed at 100% correct on the first session with 7 to 10
dots, indicating that orangutans are capable of numerical estimation for a
greater number of items than can presumably be subitized.
4:35
32-5 Discrete Quantity Discrimination
in Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)
Ursula S. Anderson1,2, Terry L. Maple1,2,
Mollie A. Bloomsmith1,2, M. J. Marr1, Tara Stoinski2,
& Anderson D. Smith1 (Georgia Institute of Technology1
& TECHlab, Zoo Atlanta2)
Two experiments with gorillas
(Gorilla gorilla gorilla) demonstrated their ability to discriminate discrete
quantities. In both experiments, we simultaneously presented each subject with
two quantities in two wells (e.g., two grapes in the 1st well and
four in the 2nd). A correct response was defined as the subject
selecting the well containing the greatest quantity. The first experiment used
a non-corrective method to test spontaneous discrimination ability. Statistical
analyses indicated that the subjects (N = 11) were discriminating between
quantities and that age, sex, subject testing order, and the ratio between the
two quantities were significant predictors of a correct response. In the second
experiment subjects were trained to respond correctly using a corrective method
and a learning criterion of 80% correct responses for two testing days.
Statistical analysis indicated that the number of testing days each subject
required to reach the criterion was not predicted by age or sex.
4:40
33-5 Numerical ability in pigeons
Tomoko Inagaki
(Hunter College/The CUNY Graduate Center and University Center)
A
series of experiments with four pigeons suggested that the pigeons could
discern quantities based on relative differences in discrete elements, and
their numerical decisions were different when a stimulus did not consist of
discrete elements, such as color filled horizontal bar stimuli. Four pigeons
trained with a row of five red rectangles and five green rectangles were then
tested with different numbers of red and green rectangles in order (Ex1) or
randomly arranged (Ex2) within a row. Accuracy decreased as the number of
same colored elements within a row decreased (Ex1). There was no significant
effect of ordinal position of different colored elements within a row (Ex2).
When tested with horizontal bar stimuli consisting of red and green color
components, the pigeons' accuracy significantly decreased
(Ex3).
4:45 Discussion of
Presentations 31, 32, & 33
4:50
34-5 Timed
performance in simple conditioning procedures
Mika MacInnis (Brown University)
The problem was to determine
whether a simple timing theory is able
to account for a rat’s performance in delay, trace, and backward conditioning
procedures. Using a Latin square design,
each of 18 rats was trained on instrumental appetitive head entry conditioning
procedures consisting of a 20-s stimulus and a 100-s ITI. Within a phase, food was available only at a
single time following stimulus onset. In
each phase, it was delivered directly after the first head entry occurring 10,
20, 30, 50, 110, or 120 s following stimulus onset. These conditions are traditionally known as
delay, trace, and backward conditioning procedures. In all cases, performance was determined by
the time from relevant events (stimulus on, stimulus termination, and food
delivery). A simple model based on the expected time to reinforcement was used
to predict the observed performance.
4:55
Paulo Guilhardi (Brown University)
Standard procedures of model
evaluation and selection are based on measures of the adequacy of models, such
as goodness-of-fit measures, calculated on all available data. Other measures
are necessary to assess the complexity and generality of a model. The
cross-validation method involves estimation of parameters of a model from some
of the data, and the use of these parameters for goodness-of-fit measures on
other data. This method is typically used to avoid overly complex models and to
establish that the model applies to other samples of the data. It may also be
used to determine whether a model is restricted to a particular dependent
variable or procedure, or whether it is a general model of a process. The
cross-validation method will be applied to evaluate the generality of the
predictions of scalar timing theory with respect to different samples,
different dependent variables, and different fixed-interval procedures.
5:00 Discussion of
Presentations 34 & 35
Individual Differences
Chair, Nestor Schmajuk
5:25
36-10 Individual
Differences in the Behavior of Rats.
Russell M. Church, Paulo Guilhardi, & Laura
Armstrong (Brown University)
Although rats often behave in
a similar manner under similar conditions, they may be readily identified by
their behavior. Twenty-four rats were trained for 20 sessions in a delayed
conditioning procedure with four different 30-s stimuli that were separated by
an interval with a mean of 2.5 min. Food was delivered at the end of two of the
stimuli, and there was no food at the end of the other two stimuli. The time of
occurrence of head entries were recorded. Although all rats developed stimulus
and temporal discriminations, reliable differences among rats were readily
identified by measuring characteristics of the performance in half the
sessions, and testing for these differences in the other half of the sessions.
These individual differences would be particularly important if reliable
patterns were identified on dimensions such as pattern and rate.
5:40
37-10 The structure of individual
differences in olfactory cognition in mice
Chuck Locurto (College of the Holy Cross)
We have previously reported
that when mice (Mus musculus) are given a battery of visio/spatial problem
solving tasks the resultant factor structure is more congenial to a modular
rather than a general solution, meaning there was no evidence for the presence
of a powerful first factor that accounted for a large proportion of matrix
variance. There are reasons to think that olfactory tasks might yield more
evidence for a general solution. In two experiments we explored the structure
of individual differences in olfactory problems that were embedded within a
battery that also contained spatial tasks. The results of both experiments
suggest that in some cases olfactory tasks possess higher covariance than is
found in spatial tasks, but it is doubtful that a general solution of the type
often found in human problem solving is appropriate for these data.
Stimulus Categories, Classes, and Patterns
Chair, Marcia Spetch
6:05
38-20 Linked perceptual classes:
Bridging perceptual and equivalence classes
Lanny Fields (Queens College and the Graduate School of
CUNY)
A linked perceptual class
exists when members of two distinct perceptual classes occasion the mutual
selection of each other. An example would be the infinite images of cats and
the sounds they emit. These classes are more complex than those typically
studied in animal cognition. Their formation has also not been studied in
humans. MTS based procedures were used to measure class formation and identify
three variables that influenced class formation by humans. Linked perceptual
class formation was an inverse function of the number of conditional
discriminations used to link the classes, an interactive function of the
particular class members used as samples and comparisons in the trained
relations that linked the classes, and an inverse negatively accelerated
function of the number of different probes presented in the test blocks that
documented class formation. Similar procedures procedures and variables might
also induce linked perceptual classes in infrahumans.
6:30
39-10 Multiple category learning in
capuchin monkeys
Satoru Ishikawa (Hokkaido University), Hika Kuroshima,
& Kazuo Fujita (Kyoto University)
Three Capuchin monkeys (Cebus
aperra) were examined to acquire four categories and two higher-order
categories with matching-to-sample (MTS) task and/or conditioned position
discrimination task. In the initial phase, monkeys acquired to discriminate
four categories having 100 exemplars in each category and transferred to the
novel 100 ones under the both training task. In the second phase, they were
trained to discriminate two higher-order categories having 100 exemplars half
of which were derived from previous four categories and the other exemplars
were prepared as new ones, using MTS task. Subjects also acquired this
discrimination and transferred to the novel exemplars. Analyzing the error
responses, it was indicated that the monkeys reconstructed boundaries among
four categories and set these categories more plausible way through acquisition
learning of the higher-order categories.
6:45
40-10 Bringing down the hierarchy: Evidence that hierarchical patterns are
processed in a linear fashion
James D. Rowan, Warren J. Neiland & Gopica Rasiah
(Bridgewater College)
Findings of an experiment that
required human subjects to learn 48 item serial patterns supported earlier
findings (Fountain & Rowan, 1995) which demonstrated that a 3 level
hierarchical pattern is learned faster than a modified version of the pattern
with the hierarchical structure violated.
The performance of four additional groups of subjects, however, brings
into question the idea that they somehow encode a representation of the
multilevel hierarchical structure.
Subjects in these groups learned serial patterns, which consisted of 2,
24 item, 2-level hierarchically structured arranged in a liner fashion. The
results suggest a more linear representation of the pattern structure is
encoded instead of the accepted structural tree representation as proposed by
Fountain and Rowan (1995) and others.
7:00
41-5 The influence of familiarity on the
recognition of object images from novel perspectives in capuchin monkeys (Cebus
apella)
Katherine A. Leighty (University of Georgia), Hika
Kuroshima (Kyoto University) & Kazuo Fujita (Kyoto University)
Two capuchin monkeys were
tested to determine the impact of object familiarity on the recognition of
object images from novel perspectives on a touchscreen monitor. Twelve novel objects served as stimuli, six
of which were presented in manipulation sessions prior to testing to make them
“familiar”. Upon achieving criterion
performance on MTS trials, subjects were presented with sample stimuli from
novel vantages and four orthogonal comparison stimuli in probe trials. Performance exceeded chance on both familiar
and unfamiliar object probe trials.
While individual subjects demonstrated the trend of improved performance
with familiar objects, neither did so significantly. When data from the two subjects were
combined, a significant effect of familiarity was found (F=258.14, p<.05). These results support previous findings with
humans and pigeons that familiarity with objects improves the ability to
recognize their images from novel perspectives.
7:05 Discussion of
Presentation 41
Funding Opportunities for Research and Teaching Programs in
Comparative Cognition:
Chair, Marcia Spetch
7:10
42-10 Opportunities
at the National Science Foundation
Fred Stollnitz (National Science Foundation)
The National Science
Foundation (www.nsf.gov) supports research
and education in comparative cognition, as it does in nearly all fields of
science that are not disease-oriented. Research in comparative cognition is
supported primarily in the Directorate for Biological Sciences; projects may be
small, single-investigator projects or large, multifaceted projects (e.g.,
Frontiers in Biological Research). Education projects may involve teacher
enhancement; course, curriculum or laboratory improvement, or informal
education of the general public through zoo or museum exhibits, films or TV
programs, etc. Projects that integrate research and education are particularly
welcome, as in Research in Undergraduate Institutions, Faculty Early Career
Development, Research on Learning and Education, Research Opportunities for
Teachers, Research Opportunities for Undergraduates, and Undergraduate
Mentoring in Environmental Biology (defined broadly enough to include
comparative cognition!). Opportunities to serve as a program officer or as a
reviewer are available in many NSF programs.
Friday Morning
Social Cognition I
Chair, Heidi Harley
8:00
43-10 Inference formation and social
cognition in Pinyon Jays
Guillermo Paz-y-Mińo C., Alan B. Bond & Alan C.
Kamil (University of Nebraska-Lincoln)
We report preliminary results
on the ability of pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) to track dyadic
dominance relationships between conspecifics (i.e. A>B, B>C) and use this
information to infer the outcome of further dominance encounters (i.e. A>C).
The data is interpreted in the context of the social complexity hypothesis
which indicates that animals that live in large, social, stable groups have
evolved substantial cognitive abilities that allow them to evaluate and
categorize individuals according to their social rank, reproductive status,
and/or foraging success.
8:15
44-10 Goal/desire attribution in human babies and infant chimpanzees
Claudia
Uller (University of Louisiana at Lafayette)
Does the chimpanzee attribute
goals to others? The question of the
capacity for theory of mind in human children and nonhuman primates continues
to haunt researchers. Two experiments with 4-12 month old infant chimpanzees
and 9-12 month old human babies show that the capacity for goal attribution may
be a precursor of theory of mind present in infant chimpanzees and infant human
babies. The experiments were closely modeled on 12 month old infant experiments
developed by Gergely and colleagues (1995). The infant chimpanzees performed as
well as human infants, which suggests that they also attribute goals and hence
that precursors of this capacity may not be distinctively human.
8:30
45-5 The influence of social context
and reward on stimulus enhancement in brown capuchin monkeys
Kristin E. Bonnie & Frans B.M. de Waal (Emory
University/Yerkes National Primate Research Center)
The influences of social
relationship and reward on social learning of a stimulus enhancement task were
investigated in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Subjects were paired
with models that varied in terms of their dominance
relationship and kinship with the subject. Subjects first observed
trained models search one of three uniquely colored and patterned boxes,
presented in randomized locations directly in front of the animal. On the next
trial, subjects were then given the opportunity to explore the same, but
randomly rearranged, boxes. The total number of responses, latency to respond
and success in matching the model’s choices were assessed under three
experimental conditions: a) neither model nor subject
is rewarded, b) only the model is rewarded, and c) both are rewarded, for
correct choices. Preliminary results reveal that significant differences
exist between reward conditions, and also suggest that the subject may not need
to be rewarded for social learning to occur.
8:35
46-5 Mind-reading and probability
learning under uncertainty: Animal triangulation paradigm applied to homo
sapiens
Kang Lee (Queen’s University), Connie Poon (Waterloo
University), & Darwin Muir (Queen’s University)
Humans’ use of gaze cues to
make predictions about probabilistic events was tested using a triangulation
paradigm commonly used to study primate mental state attribution. Participants
viewed an animated actor hiding an item under one of three cups in the presence
of two witnesses: one looking at the display (looker) and the other looking
away. Afterwards, each witness indicated
the baited cup correctly either 70% or 30% of the time. Participants guessed
the location of the bait and received feedback for 100 trials. Unlike rats and
chimpanzees studied to date, human participants were strongly biased by the
information provided by the looker over that of the non-looker for the first 70
trails, after which their responses matched the actual probability. However, in
a post-test, participants still over-estimated the probability that the looker
was correct, suggesting that when humans make judgments under uncertainty, they
use the heuristic: “seeing leads to knowing”.
8:40 Discussion of
Presentations 45 & 46
8:45
47-5 Audience
effects on food call rate and structure in brown capuchins
Amy S. Pollick, Frans B.M. de Waal & Harold
Gouzoules (Emory University)
Brown capuchins (Cebus apella)
give contextually distinct calls upon encountering food. Previous studies on other species suggest
audience effects for such calls and we predicted that capuchins would adjust
their food calling to both the amount of food and the nature of their
audience. Twelve female capuchins
underwent two food conditions (large and small amounts) and three audience
conditions (single, with one other, with group). Subjects gave more calls for larger rather
than smaller amounts of food. Subjects
also called more for a group audience over any other condition regardless of
food quantity. Preliminary acoustic
analyses indicate that certain audiences influence amplitude and pitch of the
calls in addition to rate of calling.
This suggests that food calls
take audience into account, possibly reflecting a cost/benefit calculation
related to the potential for food sharing and competition.
8:50
48-5 Courtship displays of male
pigeons can be triggered by video-taped and computer-animated pigeons
Tadd Patton1, Sylvana Yelda1,
Jens-Uwe Buschmann2, Nikolaus Troje2, & Toru Shimizu1
(1University of South Florida, 2University of
Bochum)
Male pigeons show
species-specific courtship displays in front of female pigeons. The present study examined whether visual
information, without auditory or tactile input, could trigger such courtship displays. The study also examined which technical
measures could be used to present the visual stimuli. We studied the behaviors of male pigeons in
response to video-taped and computer-animated stimuli presented on a computer
monitor. The subjects did show courtship
displays in front of video-taped and computer-animated females. However, they showed little or no such
behavior when empty cages or upside-down views of females were presented on the
monitor. Thus, subjects selectively
reacted to the visual stimuli, suggesting that the artificial pigeons can be
used as stimuli to represent a viable potential mate.
8:55 Discussion of
Presenations 47 & 48
Stimulus Value, Reinforcement, and Extinction
Chair, Jeff Katz
9:10
49-10 Reaction-time
signatures of discriminative processes: Stimulus identity versus stimulus value
Donald S. Blough (Brown University)
Experiments compared reaction
times (RTs) to stimuli differing in similarity with RTs to stimuli differing in
probability of reinforcement. On each
trial a pigeon pecked a small spot that could assume any one of 5 hues. A peck to a red S+ always brought food,
whereas pecks to any of 3 other reds, similar to S+, went unreinforced. A peck to a green spot brought food with a
probability that changed between blocks of sessions. Differential RT patterns emerged: Similarity along the red dimension affected
the number of responses emitted but left the position and shape of RT
distributions relatively constant.
Reinforcement changes to the green stimulus reversed this pattern,
shifting the RT distributions while little affecting the number of responses
emitted. RTs thus seem to distinguish
discriminative decisions that may usually be confounded: (1) "is this stimulus the same as
S+" and (2) "of what value is this stimulus."
9:25
50-10 Extinction does not occur in
humans when they are not allowed to respond during training.
Helena Matute, Sonia Vegas & Miguel A. Vadillo (Universidad de
Deusto)
Despite extinction being such a universal finding, it is not always
observed in human research. As an example, if humans are not allowed to respond
during the training phases, they show no extinction at test. This and other
similar results can be interpreted as suggesting that current learning theories
cannot explain extinction in the human species. This also suggests that
different processes could be at work when humans learn something without
responding than when they learn something while responding. Our experiments,
however, indicate that this is not the case. For example, if contextual cues
for extinction are introduced at test in the no-response condition, extinction
is observed. We suggest that the absence of extinction observed when humans are
not allowed to respond can be interpreted as a renewal effect: If the subject
is not responding during training, the test phase (requesting a response) is
perceived as a new context.
9:40
51-5 Value Transfer in Conditioned
Inhibition
Benjamin G. Simpkins, William T. Suits & Janice N.
Steirn (Georgia Southern University)
This pair of experiments tests
the associatively-based Value Transfer (VT) model in the autoshaping of
pigeons. Value transfer has been demonstrated primarily in operant choice
procedures. The current studies examine it in a classical conditioning
conditioned inhibition procedure. Previous studies on VT have shown that a
stimulus not associated with an appetitive outcome can acquire associative
value when presented alongside an appetitive stimulus. The first experiment
focuses on the transfer of appetitive value to stimuli that indicate
non-occurrence of reinforcement (conditioned inhibitors). The second experiment
tests VT to conditioned inhibitors associated with a nonpreferred food color
from stimuli associated with a preferred food color. The results are examined for both value
transfer and contrast effects.
9:45
52-5 The effect of trial spacing on extinction of Pavlovian responding
Jennifer M. Gates, Whitney B. Werstlein, Marshall G.
Miller, & James C. Denniston (Appalachian State University)
Thirsty rats were used to
investigate the effect of trial spacing on experimental extinction. Subjects received Pavlovian training intended
to condition a fear to two separate audiovisual stimuli (CSs X and Y) through
pairings with an aversive footshock.
Following acquisition training, subjects received either massed or
spaced extinction treatment of CS X.
Subjects in Group Spaced received 10 X-noUS trials during each of 5
daily 120-min extinction sessions, whereas subjects in Group Massed received
all 50 X-noUS trials during the 5th extinction session. At test, both groups exhibited strong
conditioned responding to CS Y, the nonextinguished CS. Of greater interest, behavioral testing with
CS X revealed attenuated conditioned responding to CS X in Group Spaced,
relative to Group Massed. Thus, spaced
extinction treatment produced a greater attenuation of conditioned fear than
did massed extinction treatment. Results
will be discussed in terms of contemporary theories of extinction and stimulus
processing.
9:50 Discussion of
Presentations 51 & 52
9:55
53-5 The
effect of CS duration on extinction of Pavlovian responding.
Whitney B. Werstlein, Jennifer M. Gates, Marshall G. Miller,
& James C. Denniston (Appalachian State University)
Thirsty rats were used to
investigate the effect of CS duration on the extinction of Pavlovian
responding. Subjects received Pavlovian
training intended to condition a fear to two separate audiovisual stimuli (CSs
X and Y) through pairings with an aversive footshock. Following acquisition training, subjects in
Group Short received 10 1-min exposures to CS X during each of 5 daily
sessions, whereas subjects in Group Long received a single 50-min exposure to
CS X during the 5th extinction session. At test, both groups exhibited strong
conditioned responding to CS Y, the nonextinguished CS. More importantly, testing with CS X revealed
attenuated conditioned responding to X in Group Short, relative to Group
Long. These results stand in contrast to
timing models of Pavlovian conditioning (i.e., Gallistel & Gibbon, 2000)
which propose that CS duration has no effect on the rate of experimental
extinction.
10:00
54-5 Wheel-running
under different conditions of effort: Extinction effects
Karen L. Roper & Caleb Masland (Wake Forest
University)
Male albino rats were trained
on a progressive ratio (PR4) schedule to run in a wheel for a sweetened milk
reward under one of two force requirements (7 or 30 gram loadings). Once a “breakpoint” number of revolutions had
been achieved (determined separately for each rat), both groups ran in the
wheel without reward and were compared to control rats for which running had
never been reinforced. Rats from both
groups completed equivalent ratios (modal PR at breakpoint = 24)
when reinforced; however, rats with the greater force applied to the wheel
showed the least resistance to extinction.
One explanation for this result is that rewards following increased
effort are more valued. Rats with
contrasting effort requirements are being trained to determine if the effect of
an effort comparison process would enhance this effect.
10:05 Discussion of
Presentations 53 & 54
Stimulus Identity and Same/Different Concept Learning
Chair, Ed Wasserman
10:20
55-20 Abstract-concept learning and
list-memory processing
Anthony A. Wright (University of Texas Medical School
at Houston), Jeffrey S. Katz (Auburn University), Jacquelyne J. Rivera, & Jocelyne Bachevalier (University of
Texas Medical School at Houston)
Three
capuchin (Cebus apella) monkeys were trained in a same/different
task to touch the lower of two pictures for same or a white rectangle
for different. Abstract-concept learning increased from 52% to 87% with
expansion of the training set size from 8 to 128 pictures. Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) trained similarly
were at a disadvantage, but three others trained to touch the top picture
before making a choice were similar to the capuchin monkeys. In list-memory learning, rhesus monkeys trained to touch the top picture
showed an initial advantage but later were similar to the capuchin monkeys.
Serial position functions from both monkey species were similar in shape and
changes with retention delays. Pigeons
and humans were qualitatively similar to the monkey species but showed
quantitatively different time-courses. In abstract-concept learning, the
set-size functions for capuchin monkeys, rhesus monkeys, and pigeons eventually
show complete concept learning. This
complete concept learning for the three species is evidence of qualitative
similar processes, whereas differences in the slopes of these set-size
functions are evidence of quantitative differences. Qualitative similarity is evidence for a
general-process account as opposed to a modular account of these cognitive
abilities.
10:45
56-10 Is identity special? The implicit
perception of identity/non-identity relations in adult humans
Mary Jo Rattermann,
Kirsten Keller, & Lauren Lanza (Franklin & Marshall College)
Using
the methodology developed by Wasserman and Young (Wasserman, Fagot & Young,
2001;Young & Wasserman, 2001) a touchscreen Imac was used to present human
adults with either a display of 16 identical icons or a display of 16
non-identical icons. Subjects received an equal number of identity and
non-identity trials, but were only rewarded for touching the screen in the
presence of identity (or non-identity, depending upon condition). To create an
ambiguous situation, only half of the trials that contained the relevant
identity relation were rewarded. As expected, the subjects reinforced for
responding to identity touched the screen more for identity displays. Contrary
to our expectations, the subjects reinforced for responding to non-identity
also touched the screen more for identity displays. These findings suggest a
predisposition to respond based on identity – a predisposition so strong that
subjects respond to the presence of identity, even when reinforced for non-identity.
11:00
Timothy Flemming, Anna Follensbee, Kerry McAuliffe
& Roger K. R. Thompson (Franklin and Marshall College)
Comparative investigations of
the implicit discrimination of same/different relations are confounded by
methodological differences. A procedure developed by Wasserman et al (2002) is
applicable to both primates and birds. Macaque monkeys were trained to respond
on a FI-15" schedule of reinforcement to each of four stimulus arrays
consisting of either 16 identical (same) or 16 nonidentical (different) stimuli
drawn from two separate sets of icons (Same1, Same2, Different1, &
Different2). One of the four stimulus arrays was designated as S+ for each
animal after baseline training. Changes in cumulative response frequencies to
the S+ and 3 S-s disclosed whether the animals spontaneously attended to the
physical features of individual icons or the same/different relations between
and among the displays. Results from more than fifty 48-trial sessions indicate
that the abstract relational dimensions of the stimulus arrays were less
salient for these monkeys than for pigeons on this particular task.
11:05
58-5 Abstract-concept learning differs
when vertical and horizontal stimulus panels are used in MTS by pigeons
Jeffrey S. Katz, Kent D. Bodily, Michelle Hernandez
(Auburn University), & Anthony A. Wright (University of Texas Medical
School at Houston)
Wright (1997, 2001) found
that pigeons (Columba livia) completely learned an abstract concept in a
matching-to-sample task when required to peck a sample stimulus 20 times
(FR20). When pigeons did not learn the concept (e.g., FR0 or FR1)
discrimination was controlled by configural, as opposed to if-then rule
learning. Those experiments were conducted in an operant chamber using a
horizontal panel. A replication of this study with the same stimuli and display
size but using a traditional vertical stimulus panel was conducted. Under such
conditions pigeons (FR1 or FR20) did not learn the abstract concept. Instead,
they learned by if-then rules. The implication of these findings will be
discussed.
11:10
59-5
A
display size effect on matching-to-sample strategies by pigeons
Kent D.
Bodily, Jeffrey S. Katz, (Auburn University), & Anthony A. Wright
(University of Texas Medical School at Houston)
Three
strategies that can be used to successfully perform matching-to-sample have
been examined in pigeons: if-then rule learning, configural learning, and
relational learning. The first two strategies are stimulus specific, that is,
successful performance is based on memorizing a response to each stimulus,
whereas relational learning transcends the stimuli used to train it. Using a vertical stimulus panel, our present
study used the Wright (1997) method to distinguish between strategies and
explores the effect of display size on which strategy is learned. Decreasing display size increased relational
learning.
11:15 Discussion of Presentations
57, 58, & 59
Friday Afternoon
Visual Discrimination Learning
Chair, Michael Brown
1:30
60-10 Attention to what, where, and
when in pigeons
Jennifer E. Sutton & Sara J. Shettleworth
(University of Toronto)
Given that animals have a
limited amount of attention to allocate to the large amount of information
encountered on a moment to moment basis, our experiments explored whether
spatial, temporal, and identity information compete for processing resources or
are processed in parallel. In one experiment,
pigeons were trained on a discrimination task where either location or duration
of a sample stimulus could be used to respond correctly to a subsequent
comparison stimulus. On probe trials, either the spatial or the temporal
information was made ambiguous, or the two sources of information were put in
conflict. Spatial information dominated temporal processing on probe trials,
suggesting an asymmetrical allocation of attention to the two dimensions.
Further experiments exploring other combinations of stimulus location,
duration, and identity will also be reported
1:45
61-10 A rose from
any other view is still a rose: Viewpoint effects in pigeons’ object recognition
Marcia Spetch, Alinda Friedman & Anne Ferrey
(University of Alberta)
Effective interaction with the
environment often requires the ability to recognize important objects from
different viewpoint, but the processes by which animals and humans recognize
rotated objects are not fully understood.
In previous comparative studies, we found that pigeons’ recognition of
depth-rotated objects was similar to that of humans except in two respects.
First, pigeons, unlike humans, showed as much viewpoint dependence for objects
that contained a single distinctive geon as for objects that contained multiple
geons. Second, pigeons did not show interpolation effects. As is typical of studies of object
recognition in both animals and people, pictures of objects, rather than real
objects served as the stimuli in our previous studies. In the present studies
we used real objects to explore whether object recognition processes or picture
interpretation processes, underlie the species differences.
2:00
62-5 Simple visual discrimination
training of the giant panda
Angela S. Kelling (Georgia Institute of Technology, Zoo
Atlanta), Rebecca Snyder (Zoo Atlanta), Jack Marr (Georgia Institute of
Technology), Mollie Bloomsmith (Zoo Atlanta), & Terry Maple (Georgia
Institute of Technology, Zoo Atlanta)
The giant pandas (Ailuropoda
melanoleuca) at Zoo Atlanta are
being trained on simple visual discrimination by adapting a method used to
train juvenile American black bears (Ursus
americanus). Initial training has provided insights into
giant pandas and about the challenges of training them. The subjects have demonstrated the ability to learn simple visual contrast
discriminations. Currently, the pandas
are being trained on reversal contrast discriminations and future training will
investigate their ability to learn several color discriminations.
2:05
63-5 How do
pigeons respond when presented with conflicting information?
Kumiko Yokoyama & Sheila Chase (Hunter College,
CUNY)
Four pigeons were trained on
alternate days to discriminate between two forms and two colors. After they
mastered both discrimination tasks a form and a color were presented together
on probe trials during regular training sessions. When the color and the form
required different responses, the birds tended to respond on the basis of
color. This was true for both color and form sessions.
2:10
64-5 Examination of global and local
visual processing in pigeons and humans
Kazuhiro Goto & Stephen E. G. Lea (University of
Exeter School of Psychology)
Two experiments have been
conducted in pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens) to examine the
relative precedence of global and local visual processing. First, precedence was examined by comparing
acquisition rates for a categorization of geometric hierarchical stimuli. Categorization based on global features was
achieved faster than that based on local features in both species
examined. The second experiment examined
global and local processing in the discrimination of two perceptually similar
categories, cats and dogs. Following
acquisition, the stimuli were manipulated in two different ways: mosaicisation
conserved some global information but lost high frequency information, whereas
scrambling conserved high frequency but lost global features. Despite the global precedence found in the previous
experiment, humans recognized scrambled cats better than mosaicized cats though
the reverse was true for dogs. Results
from pigeons, unlike humans, indicated that they recognized mosaicized images
better than scrambled images of both dogs and cats.
2:15 Discussion of
Presentations 62, 63, & 64
2:20
65-5 Directional motion categorization
by pigeons
Angie Koban, Robert Cook (Tufts University), & Joel
Fagot (Center for Research in Cognitive Neurosciences)
Six pigeons were trained to
discriminate between videos of object stimuli rotating either left or right
around a central axis. The objects consisted of a red cube, tube, cone, torus,
or differently organized groups of small disks. The animals readily learned the
directional discrimination and transferred to changes in surface
characteristics, but showed little direct transfer to new objects. Featural
versus object-based interpretations of these motion data will be discussed.
2:25
66-5 Pigeon’s recognition of static
and dynamic images of human faces
Hiroshi Makino(Chiba University)
In Experiment 1, pigeons were trained to discriminate frontal views of two
positive and two negative human faces in a go/no-go discrimination procedure
and then tested for transfer to novel viewpoints over the range of ±90°. The
pigeons showed substantial transfer to the novel views, but their responses
decreased systematically as the positive faces were rotated away from the
trained view. In Experiment 2, the pigeons were trained to discriminate
one positive and one negative face dynamically rotating in depth. Their
initial performances revealed the failure of transfer from the static to
dynamic views. Although the pigeons eventually learned to discriminate the
dynamic stimuli, the dynamic-view training failed to broaden the range of
testing performance with the static stimuli, thus there was no indication of
transfer between the static and dynamic views. With naďve pigeons as
subjects, transfer from the dynamic to static stimuli was reevaluated in
Experiment 3.
2:30
67-5 Object localization in picture
stimuli by pigeons
P. Taylor Johnson & Robert Cook (Tufts University)
A new procedure for exploring
object perception in pigeons was described.
Pigeons were trained to locate a target “object” within a larger picture
of assorted objects in a conditional discrimination procedure. Preliminary data
for task acquisition and transfer will be presented. Implications for object perception and visual
cognition in birds will be discussed.
2:35 Discussion of
Presentations 65, 66, & 67
Spatial Learning and Spatial Cues
Chair, Jerry Cohen
2:55
68-10 Effects of carrying food versus
encountering food on caching and retrieval behavior in rats
William A. Roberts, Tammy L.B. McKenzie, Leanne R.
Bird, & Jason Rice (University of Western Ontario)
The ability to accurately
retrieve hidden food items was compared on a radial maze in two groups of rats,
one that cached food and one that encountered food. Rats that cached food
learned to retrieve it accurately faster than rats that encountered it. In a
second experiment, all cached or encountered food was pilfered before the
retrieval test. Rats that cached food continued both to cache and to visit
cache sites before non-cache sites, but rats that encountered food lost their
preference for cache locations. These findings provide support for the argument
that the behavior of carrying food to hidden locations on the radial maze and
later retrieving it involves a biologically specialized module.
3:10
69-10 Detour behavior in the quokka
(Setonix brachyurus)
Clive D. L. Wynne (University of Florida) & Benoit
Leguet (Ecole Polytechnique)
Four wild-living quokkas (an
herbivorous macropod marsupial) were tested for their ability to find a goal by
progressing around a transparent barrier. Barriers tested were either
symmetrical or L-shaped. Left-right location of the arm of L-shaped barriers
was randomized. One quokka chose the shorter route around an L-shaped barrier
on first exposure (spatial reasoning); the other three gradually acquired a
tendency to select the shorter route (spatial learning). These three quokka
also showed a characteristic preference to turn either left or right around the
L barrier. This preference declined as the side-arm of the barrier was extended
from 70 cm to 2.1 m.
3:25
70-10 Spatial Learning in Cephalopods
Jean Geary Boal (Millersville University),
Miranda A. Karson (Michigan State University) & Roger T. Hanlon (Marine
Biological Laboratory)
Field data suggest that cephalopods
are capable of spatial learning. To investigate this learning explicitly, maze
experiments were performed using Octopus
bimaculoides and Sepia officinalis
as subjects. Octopus movements within a novel arena decreased with time,
consistent with an interpretation of exploratory learning. Octopuses remembered
the location of an open burrow after 24 h away from the test arena. In a
six-choice, open-field maze, octopuses learned the location of a burrow,
retained that memory for at least a week, and showed savings in learning a 180o
reversal task. Cuttlefish also appeared to explore a novel arena, and learned
to exit a simple runway maze. Cuttlefish learned to exit a two-choice maze
using either right/left or pattern cues with equal facility, and showed improvement
in learning over a series of reversals. We conclude that both octopuses and
cuttlefish show exploratory behavior, learning, and retention of spatial
information.
3:40
71-5 Spatial orientation and landmark
use in black-capped chickadees
Bridgette A. Szekeres & David F. Sherry (University
of Western Ontario)
A series of experiments
examined landmark use by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Birds were trained to locate food at the
midpoint between two identically shaped but differently coloured landmarks 40cm
apart. Search was analysed on probe trials in which no food was available. In the first experiment, chickadees searched
accurately in the geometric centre between the two landmarks at the 40cm
inter-landmark training distance. At
novel inter-landmark distances of 10cm and 70cm, the birds adjusted their
search patterns and continued to search at the geometric centre between the
landmarks. In a further experiment, one or other of the landmarks was removed. On probe trials, birds consistently searched
on the same side of the single available landmark, suggesting they did not
distinguish the landmarks by colour but did treat members of the landmark pair
differently for distance estimation.
3:45
72-5 Proactive interference in the recall
of serially presented spatial items by Clark’s nutcrackers, Nucifraga
columbiana
Jody L. Lewis & Alan C. Kamil (University of
Nebraska Lincoln)
We tested the spatial memory
of Clark’s nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) after presentation of either one
or two lists of locations and obtained evidence for proactive
interference. Performance was better
following one list than following two lists.
When items in the two lists were located close to each other,
nutcrackers made errors by visiting first list locations during recall of the
second list.
3:50
73-5 Benefits of performing learning flights in honeybees, Apis mellifera
Cynthia A. Wei & Fred C.
Dyer (Michigan State University)
Upon discovering new sources of food, honeybees perform learning flights to memorize visual landmarks that can guide their return. The durations of such flights vary and are influenced by various factors. These include prior experience at the food source, delay between arrival at the location and receipt of food, sucrose concentration of the food, and the visual complexity and stability of spatial relationships of features in the surrounding environment. The modulation of learning flights seems to reflect a trade-off between the costs of performing a learning flight and the gains in foraging efficiency conferred by improved spatial foraging patterns. A series of experiments explores the relationship between performance of learning flights and accuracy in spatial patterns of foraging and reveals a positive correlation. Effects of learning flight duration are also explored and may suggest that longer flights lead to increased accuracy in pinpointing the location of food upon the bee's return.
3:55 Discussion of Presentations 71, 72, & 73
4:00
74-5 Effects of pilfering, food degradation
and food devaluation on food carrying and retrieval in rat.
Tammy L.B. McKenzie, Leanne R. Bird, Jason Rice, &
William A. Roberts (University of Western Ontario)
Ten naďve male Long-Evans rats
were used to examine the effects of pilfering, substituting a less preferred
food, and degrading food on food carrying and retrieval behavior on an
eight-arm radial maze. Food carried to one side of the maze was left unaltered,
whereas food on the other side was altered by one of the three methods described
above. It was found that rats did not come to prefer hoarding on the side of
the maze that was unaltered versus the side that was altered. During retrieval,
rats returned to arms that had not been altered before arms that had been
altered. These findings lend support to the idea of a food carrying and
retrieval module.
4:05
75-5 The effects of presentation order
and difficulty level on the ability of capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) to solve
two-dimensional mazes
Erica A. Hoy & Dorothy M. Fragaszy (University of
Georgia)
The planning abilities of
capuchin monkeys were investigated by examining the kind and number of errors
produced while solving two-dimensional computer mazes. Three maze-naďve monkeys were presented with
16 maze sets. Difficulty level of the mazes was a function of the total number
of choice points and the number of “non-obvious” choices. Each maze set
consisted of 12 novel mazes of varied difficulty that were presented in random
order. Errors made by these subjects were compared with errors made by three
capuchins that were previously presented with the same mazes in order of
increasing difficulty. The performance
of capuchins in the random order condition will also be compared to data
collected previously from chimpanzees in order to determine if cross-species
differences in planning abilities are present.
4:10
76-5
Chimpanzees and young children face
similar difficulties using scale models in a search task
Valerie Kuhlmeier (Yale University)
Many recent studies have
explored young children’s ability to use information from physical
representations of space to guide search within the real world. In one commonly used procedure, children are
asked to find a hidden toy in a room after observing a smaller toy being hidden
in the analogous location in a scale model of the room. Children at 2.5-years often have difficulty
with this task, while 3-year-olds readily find the hidden toy. The present series of three experiments
examined the causes of 2.5-year-olds’ difficultly with the scale model task,
using task procedures previously designed for tests with chimpanzee subjects
(Pan troglodytes). Results indicate that
the poor performance stems from a difficulty recognizing the model/room
representational relationship and,
similar to chimpanzees, from the use of an alternative, perseverative search
strategy.
4:15 Discussion of
Presentations 74, 75, & 76
Problem Solving and Tool Use
Chair, Mary Jo Rattermann
Finch
physics: Cognitive abilities related to tool-use in the woodpecker finch
Cactopiza pallida
Woodpecker finches are famous for their tool-use behaviour. They use twigs or
cactus spines to pry arthropods out of crevices and use this ability more than any other tool-using
species hitherto known. In the present study we investigated the cognitive
abilities related to tool-use in a comparative, experimental approach. We chose
3 experimental designs that have been used in earlier studies to test several
primate species (the trap-tube task and a modification task) and New Caledonia
Crows ( tool length task). One out of 6 woodpecker finch was able to solve the
trap-tube task and several individuals modified tools and chose twigs of
appropriate length. Most of our individuals learned these new tasks quickly
but we found no clear evidence for a
mental representation of the underlying physical problems. The findings for
primates and New Caledonian in theses task were very much alike. We therefore
conclude that the cognitive demands imposed by tool-use seem to result in similar learning mechanisms
among these species.
4:55
78-10 Folk physics for crows: do New
Caledonian crows understand gravity?
Jackie Chappell, Alex A. S. Weir, Ben Kenward &
Alex Kacelnik (University of Oxford)
When
humans use tools, knowledge about the properties of materials and fundamental
facts about physics guide our behaviour. However, animals lacking these
insights could generate tool behaviour that looks superficially identical. One
female New Caledonian crow was tested on a trap tube task, similar to those
used with primates. The subject learned to retrieve food successfully in 100% of
trials, within a number of trials equivalent to that required by primates,
despite the fact that the task was more complicated
(the tool could be used either to pull or push the food from either end
of the tube). She also developed her own, reliable technique to remove the
food, by combining two actions. However, the subject did not alter the
frequency of her responses when the trap tube was inverted, suggesting that she
had learnt to displace food away from the trap without reference to the
principle of gravity.
5:10
79-5 Hook shaping
in New Caledonian crows
Alex A. S. Weir, Jackie Chappell, & Alex Kacelnik
(Oxford University)
In an experiment investigating
tool choice in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides), one subject
spontaneously modified the shape of the inappropriate tool to form a hook, and
used this successfully in the task. The
initial task, which the subjects were familiar with, was to retrieve a bucket
containing meat from the bottom of a vertical Perspex tube. Two pieces of wire were provided: one
straight (the inappropriate tool, since it could not be used to retrieve the
bucket) and one hooked. In the fifth
trial of the experiment, the male subject removed the hooked wire, and the
female subject subsequently bent the straight one into a hook and used this
successfully. When later presented with
straight wire only, she repeated this behavior on 9/10 trials. This is the first demonstration of
spontaneous, novel tool modification without trial-and-error learning in any
non-human.
5:15
80-5 Ignorant rhesus monkeys collect
information before acting
Robert R. Hampton & Aaron Zivin (National Institute
of Mental Health)
Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)
were allowed to choose among four possible locations where a candy could be
hidden. The location containing the candy varied randomly from trial to trial.
On a given trial, the monkeys either observed the placement of the candy or
were kept ignorant by blocking their view during baiting. Most monkeys that
knew the location of the candy chose that location without hesitation. In
contrast, when ignorant of the location, monkeys often made the effort required
to learn the location before choosing. This difference in behavior between
trials on which the monkeys knew, and trials on which they were ignorant, is
consistent with the hypothesis that they can discriminate between knowing and
not knowing.
5:20 Discussion of
Presentations 79 & 80
Associative Learning and Cue Competition
Chair, Aaron Blaisdell
5:35
81-20 Biologically significant cues are
relatively immune to cue competition
Ralph R. Miller (SUNY-Binghamton)
We define “biologically
significant” cues as stimuli that control or did control behavior because they
have or have had either inherent or acquired motivational value. We will review data demonstrating that
biologically significant target cues are relatively immune to the induction of
cue competition through manipulation of other stimuli that were present while
it was being trained (i.e., companion cues; e.g., overshadowing, forward
blocking, backward blocking, relative stimulus validity effect, and degraded
contingency effect), whereas target cues lacking biological significance are
subject to all of these effects. In
contrast to this asymmetry in indirect effects, behavioral control by the
target cue is readily attenuated through direct treatment, such as
reinforcement, extinction, and conditioned inhibition training. New data will be presented in which cues,
that were trained in compound with a companion stimulus, were perfectly matched
other than for their biological significance.
Posttraining decreases in the associative status of the target cue’s
companion stimulus increased the associative status of the target cue
regardless of the biological significance of the target cue, whereas
posttraining increases in the associative status of the target cue’s companion
stimulus decreased the associative status of the target cue only if the target
cue was of low biological significance.
We will suggest a functional interpretation of this asymmetry.
6:00
82-10 Responding to a blocking stimulus
is reduced by compound reinforced training
Francisco Arcediano, Martha Escobar (Auburn
University), & Ralph R. Miller (Binghamton University)
Traditional associative
learning theories assume that stimulus competition (e.g., blocking; X-US, then
XY-US results in attenuated responding to Y) reflects a deficit in acquiring an
association between Y and the US, and that the associative strength of Y cannot
be changed without additional Y-US trials.
This perspective is challenged by models that assume that stimulus
competition is a performance, rather than an acquisition deficit, as well as
acquisition models that allow modifications in the associative strength of
absent stimuli. Consistent with a
performance account, we found in a blocking paradigm that responding to both Y
and X are mutually attenuated due to their having been trained in
compound.
6:15
83-10 Irrelevant stimulation
interpolated between preexposure and conditioning disrupts latent inhibition in
a conditioned fear preparation with rat subjects
Martha Escobar, Francisco Arcediano (Auburn
University), & Ralph R. Miller (Binghamton University)
Escobar, Arcediano, and Miller
(2002) suggested that obtaining latent inhibition in unmasked tasks with adult
human participants has proven difficult because the usual human procedure
involves interpolating instructions between preexposure and conditioning. Potentially, this disruption is related to
interpolation of verbal information or to cognitive processes unique to human
participants. The present studies use a
nonhuman (rat) analog of Escobar et al.’s human procedure in a conditioned fear
preparation. We observed that
interpolation of audiovisual stimuli disrupted latent inhibition. Apparently, the interpolated stimulation
makes the context of preexposure and of conditioning different, attenuating
latent inhibition through mechanisms potentially similar to those underlying
attenuation of latent inhibition when the context is changed between phases.
6:30
84-5 Dissociation of the effects of
interpolating a retention interval versus instructions between preexposure and conditioning
in a human unmasked latent inhibition paradigm
Tyson Platt, Martha Escobar (Auburn University), &
Ralph R. Miller (Binghamton University)
Numerous reports suggest that
obtaining latent inhibition in human adults requires the use of a masking (i.e.,
distracting) task. However, Escobar,
Arcediano, and Miller (2002) observed latent inhibition in human adults using
an unmasked task. Obtaining latent
inhibition in these conditions required that the preexposure and conditioning
phases occur without interruption. Such
interruptions are usually necessary to provide participants with instructions
regarding the requirements of the conditioning task. The present study dissociates the effects of
interpolating instructions between preexposure and conditioning from the
effects of a simple retention interval between phases. Our results suggest that interpolation of
instructions disrupts latent inhibition to a greater extent than interpolation
of a retention interval.
6:35
Discussion of Presentation 84
Saturday Afternoon
Biological Substrates of Cognitive Processes
Chair, Ron Weisman
1:40
85-20 Cognitive flexibility in birds
Shigeru Watanabe (Keio University)
Cognitive flexibility was
analysed from three points. 1) Application of knowledge to new situation. 2)
Modification of knowledge through experience. 3) Flexible knowledge. The
example of the first point is demonstrated by concept discrimination by quails.
The modification of knowledge is demonstrated by experiments with scrub jays.
The flexible knowledge is demonstrated by pigeon experiments. Finally, brain mechanisms of cognitive
flexibility particularly flexible
cognition are examined by lesions of Wulst-LPO system in pigeons.
2:05
86-10 Effects of
cannabinoids on extinction of place conditioning
Linda A. Parker (Wilfrid Laurier University) &
Raphael Mechoulam (Hebrew University of Jerusalem)
Recent evidence suggests that
CB1 knockout mice display impaired extinction of Pavlovian fear
conditioning. In a series of experiments,
we explored the potential of the two principle components of marijuana, the
psychoactive cannabinoid, D-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), and the nonpsychoactive cannabinoid,
cannabidiol (CBD), to modulate the extinction of place preference and place aversion
learning. Both cannabinoids potentiated extinction of place conditioning
without producing conditioning on their own.
Our results support previous reports that the endogenous cannabinoid
system plays a role in the modulation of extinction.
2:20
87-5 Cannabinoid
modulation of sensitivity to time
Jonathon D. Crystal, Kenneth W. Maxwell, & Andrea
G. Hohmann (University of Georgia)
We examined the effect of
cannabinoids on temporal processing.
Rats trained to discriminate 2- and 8-s (Experiment 1, n=72) and 4- and 16-s (Experiment 2, n=60) intervals were tested with
intermediate durations. Psychophysical
functions (p(long) vs. duration) were characterized by measures of central
tendency (point of subjective equality, PSE) and variability (Weber fraction,
WF). The potent cannabinoid agonist,
WIN55,212-2 (1-3 mg/kg), produced a dose-related decrease in sensitivity to
time (i.e., increase in WF) without systematically affecting PSE (Experiments 1
and 2). The central cannabinoid CB1
antagonist, SR141716A (1-3 mg/kg), did not alter WF or PSE (Experiments 1 and
2). Coadministration of SR141716A with
WIN55,212-2 blocked the effect of the agonist on WF (Experiment 2), suggesting
that the WF effect is mediated by actions at cannabinoid CB1 receptors. Simulations with an information-processing
theory of timing suggest that the reduction in sensitivity to time can be
attributed to a disorder of attention.
2:25
88-5 Reversal learning after lesions in
the presumptive nucleus accumbens in pigeons
Scott Husband & Toru Shimizu (University of South
Florida)
The nucleus accumbens plays a
critical role in attention and reinforcement in mammals. In birds, chemistry and connection studies
suggest that the medial part of the lobus parolfactorius (LPO) is the avian
equivalent of the nucleus accumbens.
However, little is known about the function of this area since only a
few behavioral studies in birds have been conducted. In the present study, lesions were made in
the medial LPO of pigeons to examine behavioral effects. Pigeons with lesions did learn to
successfully discriminate visual patterns.
However, when they were tested on reversal learning of the
discrimination task, they had more errors than the control animals. The results are consistent with the notion
that the medial LPO is functionally analogous to the mammalian nucleus
accumbens.
2:30
89-5 Medial frontal cortex lesions and
rat serial pattern learning
Denise P. A. Smith & Stephen B. Fountain (Kent
State University)
Recent research in our lab has
shown that MK-801, an NMDA receptor antagonist, disrupts serial pattern
learning. Rats learned a
highly-structured serial response pattern consisting of eight 3-element chunks
with one element that violated pattern structure. Rats treated with MK-801 learned within-chunk
elements as fast as controls, but showed apparently permanent inability to
learn the violation response and, to a lesser degree, chunk boundaries. In a follow-up study, we found that dorsal
hippocampal lesions do not produce these profound deficits. In the present study, rats received medial
frontal cortex lesions, and then were trained on the same pattern as in the
earlier studies. Medial frontal rats
learned all elements of their pattern as fast as controls. The results of this andearlier studies
indicate that hippocampus and medial frontal cortex play little role in
acquisition of this task, and that other structures impacted by MK-801 are
critical for pattern acquisition.
2:35 Discussion of
Presentations 87, 88, & 89
Natural Selection and Cognition
Chair, Sara Shettleworth
2:55
90-20 Natural History and Cognition
Alan C. Kamil
& Alan B. Bond (University of Nebraska, Lincoln)
The major theme of our laboratory is to integrate psychological,
evolutionary and ecological approaches in our studies of animal cognition. This leads to a very broad-ranging research
program with implications for several disciplines. At the current time, we are concentrating on
three problems, spatial cognition in seed-caching corvids, social cognition in
group-living corvids and the detection of cryptic prey by blue jays (Cyanocitta
cristata). In this talk I will
concentrate on our cryptic prey work, demonstrating how the study of mechanism
and function can be integrated in a series of studies in which the
psychological characteristics of predators strongly influence the evolution of
their (virtual) prey.
3:20
91-20 The evolution of the cognitive
map
Lucia Jacobs (University of California at Berkeley)
& Francoise Schenk (University of Lausanne)
How the vertebrate brain sees,
understands and maps its external world is an important question in comparative
cognition. The parallel map theory
offers a novel explanation for the evolution, function and neural basis of the
cognitive map in vertebrates. In this
theory, we propose that the mammalian hippocampus maps space with two
independent representations that are mediated by two hippocampal structures,
the 'old' (dentate gyrus) and the 'new' (CA1, Ammon's horn). Because the bearing and sketch maps work in
parallel, the impairment of one map reveals residual learning by the
other. The coactivation of these
parallel maps leads to the cognitive map, an emergent property of this
activity. The parallel map theory
integrates evidence from the physiology and structure, development, ecology and
evolutionary history of the hippocampus into a unified theory of hippocampal
function.
Social Cognition II
Chair, Alan Kamil
4:10
92-10 Gaze following in ravens
Thomas Bugnyar (University of Vermont), Mareike Stöwe (KLF Gruenau) & Bernd Heinrich (University of
Vermont)
Experiments with ravens
(Corvus corax) demonstrate for the first time that birds are able to follow the
gaze (i.e. head and eye direction) of other individuals. Hand-raised ravens
were confronted with a human experimenter looking up vs. looking at the bird
(control a), and looking behind an opaque wall that blocks the view of the bird
vs. looking in the opposite direction (control b). Juveniles visually
co-orientated with the experimenter’s look-ups significantly more often than in
the control. As subadults, birds also repositioned themselves to follow the
experimenter’s looks behind the wall significantly more often than in both
controls. This suggests that the ravens’ gaze following is not a simple
behavioral response but depends on the position of the involved individuals,
i.e. whether a bird can or cannot see. This raises the question whether ravens
have some appreciation of the mental state of others.
4:25
93-10 Multisensory perception of social
signals in monkeys and birds
Sarah Partan, Sylvana Yelda, Virginia Price, & Toru
Shimizu (University of South Florida)
Multisensory communication
signals, such as vocalizations accompanied by visual postures, can be
classified by whether the components of the signal are redundant or
nonredundant. The degree of redundancy
can be determined by examining the behavioral response of subjects to
presentations of the signals in their whole (multisensory) or component
(unisensory) form. The responses of
rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and Carneaux pigeons (Columba livia)
to multi- and unisensory signals of their conspecifics will be discussed. Aggressive and affiliative multisensory
signals of macaques were studied in a naturalistic habitat in Puerto Rico. The pigeons are housed in a laboratory and
are currently the subjects of a video playback study to examine the effects of
visual and vocal components of male courtship signals on female responsiveness
4:40
94-5 Observational learning includes a
social component in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)
Sarah F.
Brosnan (Emory University) & Frans B. M. de Waal (Emory University)
To tease apart if brown
capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)
learn information socially (requiring a conspecific model) or observationally
(from information in the environment), we tested if capuchins could learn the
value of novel tokens in a familiar task, either by watching a conspecific
perform a series of 10 exchanges with each token, or by watching each token
paired the same number of times with its associated reward. Apart from the
presence or absence of a conspecific, both procedures were the same. Capuchins
did learn to favor the higher value token when watching a partner exchange, but
not if a partner was absent. Thus, in this task the involvement of a
conspecific helped capuchins learn observationally. We speculate that this is due to increased
attention towards objects being manipulated by conspecifics rather than a lack
of information in the non-social situation.
4:45
95-5 Observational learning in African
Grey Parrots
Robert J. Willson, Aaron Olivera (The University of
Greenwich) & Andrew Whiten (The University of St. Andrews)
An experiment using a domesticated
demonstrator and a wild-caught observer showed that African Grey Parrots are
capable of learning object and response choices by observing the choices of a
conspecific. Using a variation of the
two-object-two-action task, the demonstrator was trained to choose a particular
object and perform a particular directional response on that object. Once the task had been mastered, the
demonstrator performed the task in view of the observer for a series of four
sessions. Each session consisted of five
correct object and directional choices.
The observer was tested following the third and fourth session, with
each test consisting of five trials. The
results showed a significant tendency for the observer to match both the object
and directional choices of the demonstrator.
Although the results do not constitute a demonstration of 'true'
imitation, they are consistent with the idea that parrots can learn from each
other by observation.
4:50
96-5 Interspecies attention reading in
wild vervet monkeys
Sayaka Tsutsumi, Kazuo Fujita (Kyoto University), &
Jason Mwenda (Institute of Primate Research, National Museums of Kenya)
There are two categories of
wild animal species in the world; one that choose their habitat near human
residence, and the other that avoid contact with humans. The former need to know the following: 1)
cost and benefit of choosing humans as a neighbor, 2) when and where it is safe
/ dangerous to get in contact with humans, and 3) how to predict human behavior,
or, how to read the attention of humans.
Here we examined how wild
vervet monkeys, a primate species known to live closely with humans, read the
attention of human neighbor using a field experiment method. The task was to take a piece of food which
was located in front of a human actor being engaged in 4 different behaviors
with different directions / qualities of attention. The results showed that the vervet monkeys
behave differently according to the direction and quality of human’s attention.
4:55
Discussion of Presentations
94, 95, & 96
Causal reasoning
Chair, Tom Zentall
5:10
97-10 Transitive responding in pigeons
with reinforcement history controlled
Olga F. Lazareva & Edward A. Wasserman (University
of Iowa)
Solving a transitive inference
(TI) problem requires the ability to deduce that if b > c and c > d,
then b > d. In our previous research, we found that crows
responded transitively only when post-choice feedback stimuli were
dimensionally ordered. In the present
study, we tested pigeons on TI tasks involving dimensionally ordered or
non-ordered feedback stimuli. As with
crows, the pigeons could see the feedback stimuli only after their choice
between the primary discriminative stimuli.
For half of the birds the feedback stimuli were ordered by size, whereas
for the other half of the birds they were not.
Special training further guaranteed that Stimulus D had a richer
reinforcement history than Stimulus B. Nevertheless,
the pigeons strongly preferred B over D regardless of the nature of the
feedback stimuli. Follow-up simulations
showed that these results cannot be explained by linear operator models or
value transfer theory.
5:25
98-10 Transposition in pigeons
Olga F. Lazareva & Edward A. Wasserman (University
of Iowa)
In Experiment 1, we trained
four pigeons to discriminate two pairs of white circles of different
diameters: 1+ 2- and 5+ 6-, or 1- 2+ and
5- 6+ (where larger numbers correspond with larger diameters and plus and minus
signs denote reinforcement and nonreinforcement). In Experiment 2, we trained six pigeons to
discriminate four pairs of circles: 1+
2-, 1+ 3-, 4+ 6-, and 5+ 6-, or 1- 2+, 1- 3+, 4- 6+, and 5- 6+. In both experiments, testing included five
new pairings—1/5, 2/3, 2/6, 3/4, and 4/5—that clearly distinguish absolute and
relational theories of transposition.
Pigeons responded relationally to all of the test pairings. Simulations showed that neither absolute nor
relational theory could fully explain these pigeons’ data. But, a weighted average yielded a remarkably
good fit, suggesting that both relational and absolute factors may govern
pigeons’ choice behavior, with relational control greatly exceeding absolute
control.
5:40
99-10 What Accumulates in Causal
Reasoning?
J.W. (Bill) Whitlow, Jr. & Kathleen Brogan (Rutgers
University-Camden)
Associative learning models
have been used successfully to predict a number of results in human causal
reasoning, but several types of evidence raise questions about a key underlying
assumption of such theories, namely, that there is an accumulation of
associative strength or information.
However, it is difficult to interpret empirical data on information
accumulation without a theory of how information is represented. Using the connectionist modeling approach of
LEABRA, we describe our initial explorations in developing a unified account of
the role of causal relations in animal conditioning and the role of
associations in human causal reasoning.
5:55
100-10 Dogs do not
understand means-end connections via a string
Britta Osthaus, Stephen E.G. Lea & Alan M. Slater
(University of Exeter, UK)
Domestic dogs (Canis lupus)
were tested in three experiments for their understanding of means-end
connections. Results showed that dogs can solve the problem of pulling food
into their reach as long as the connecting string is laid out in a straight
line. When the string is laid out in an acute angle with the barrier the dogs
produce so-called proximity errors, i.e. they ignore the accessible end of the
string and try to reach the food instead (Experiment 1). Given the choice
between a baited and a non-baited string, the animals chose the baited one
significantly more often than the other one (Experiment 2). In the third
experiment, a combination of Experiments 1 and 2, the dogs were tested with two
crossed strings, of which only one was baited. They performed below chance
level, which indicates that they were unable to understand the means-end
properties of the connections
6:10
101-10 Causal understanding by
chimpanzees: The trap-tube revisited
Jill E. Koehler (Arizona State University), Carmen J.
Owens (The Ohio State University) & Sarah T. Boysen (The Ohio State
University)
Nine chimpanzees were tested
for an understanding of causality using a trap-tube task. The apparatus
consisted of a transparent tube with a hole (or trap) in the middle. To obtain
a reward, the chimp had to insert a wooden dowel into the end farthest from the
reward and push the candy out. Five subjects reached criterion. As a control,
the tube was rotated, with the hole now on top. Two chimps employed a new
strategy suggesting understanding of causality between use and outcome. The same subjects were tested further by
flipping the tube back with the original position. Both chimpanzees were again successful with the trap tube. Their ability
to revert to the correct strategy suggested an appreciation of causality over a
distance-based strategy.
HAPPY 10th
Anniversary CO3 !!