Animal Cognition
Home Page
History
Intelligence
Perception
Stimulus
Control
Space
& Time
Classical
Conditioning
Operant
Conditioning
Complex
Behavior
General
Links
Dr.
Robert Cook's Home Page
|
Morgan's comments on the need to see the
process leading to an "intelligent" behavior |
Excerpted from C. L.
Morgan's book The Animal Mind
In the north
of England competitions are not uncommon where, say, three sheep have to be driven over a
definite course, between certain posts and round others, through narrow passages and into
a small fold- all within a certain time limit. At such a competition success
depends on two things; first, the training of the dog to respond at once, say, to some six
or eight whistle signals, often accompanied by gestures; and secondly, the judgement of
the shepherd. The signals with different inflections have for the dog meaning, such as
drive straight on, from this side, from that, stop, lie down, creep forward, and so forth.
The dogs whole business is to obey these signals. And the instant response of a
well-trained dog is admirable. But throughout he is merely the executant of his
masters orders. He originates no important step. And if you listen to the criticisms
of the other shepherd during a competition you will find that they are mainly passed on
the judgement shown by the master, and only, in palpable failures in obedience, on the
behavior of the dog. The intelligent animal is what he is trained to be one whose
natural powers are under the complete control of his master, with whom the whole plan of
action lies.
In criticism of this statement I should now say that it
seems to imply that there was no plan in the dogs mind, whereas the more cautious
inference is that, for success from the masters point of view, and to attain his end in
view, the dogs plan must be subordinate to his plan. That, however, is not the
point. The point now is that the trainer get some insight into what, as he believes, goes
on the dogs mind, not on one occasion only but during a long series of occasions.
But since the shepherd is presumably not a trained psychologist, he bases what he tells us
on commonsense belief as to the nature of the dogs mind.
Let us now introduce the psychologist in some measure
trained to the task of interpreting mental processes. He starts with some instance of
behavior, preferably observed by himself, and tries to get behind it so as to tell us
what, as he believes, goes on in the mind of the animal.
The way in which my dog learnt to lift the latch of the
garden gate and thus let himself out was on this wise. The iron gate is held to be a
latch, but swings open by its own weight if the latch be lifted. Whenever he wanted to go out the fox terrier raised the latch with
the back of his head, and thus released the gate, which swung open. Now the question in
any such case is: How did he learn the trick? In this particular case the question can be
answered, because he was carefully watched. When he was put outside the door, he naturally
wanted to go out into the road, where there was much to tempt him the chance of a
run, other dogs to sniff at, possibly cats to be worried. He gazed eagerly out through the
railings in the low parapet wall shown in the illustration [A.B. 145, Fig. 21]; and in due
time chanced to gaze out under the latch, lifting it with his head. He withdrew his head
and looked out elsewhere; but the gate had swung open. Here was a fortunate occurrence
arising out of natural tendencies of a dog. But the association between looking out just
there and the open gate with a free passage into the road is somewhat indirect. The
coalescence of mental processes in a conscious situation effective for the guidance of
behavior did not spring into being at once. Only after some ten or twelve experiences, in
each of which the exit was more rapidly made, with less gazing out at wrong places, had
the fox terrier learnt to go straight and without hesitation to the right spot. In this
case the lifting of the latch was unquestionably hit on by accident, and the trick was
only rendered habitual by repeated association in the same situation of the chance act and
the happy escape. Once firmly established, however, the behavior remained constant
throughout the remainder of the dogs life, some five or six years. And, I may add, I could
not succeed, not withstanding much expenditure of biscuits, in teaching him to lift the
latch more elegantly with his muzzle instead of the back of his head.
Here again I might now express my interpretation more
cautiously. What I wish to stand out is this: that in some sense of the words Tony had, as
I believe, an end in view, or objective, namely to get out into the road; but
that he had not at the outset, before gazing out there, there and elsewhere, discovered
the means by which this end in view could be attained. Now the outcome of the completed
behavior, lifting the latch and getting out into the road, was such as anyone who chanced
to pass by might readily observe. I sought to ascertain through what progressive steps
this outcome was reached. The point here is that observation on one occasion only, no
matter how careful and exact that observation may be, does not suffice for the
interpretation of this or that instance of animal behavior.
Romanes'
procedures for collecting anecdotes
Romanes'
Psychological Criteria for Mind
Edward
Thorndike's criticisms of Romanes' anecdotal methodology
Charles Darwin on mental continuity between humans and animals
|